Open Access
25 July 2017 Effect of cerebral vasomotion during physical exercise on associative memory, a near-infrared spectroscopy study
Blanca Marin Bosch, Aurélien Bringard, Giulio Ferretti, Sophie Schwartz, Kinga Iglói
Author Affiliations +
Abstract
Regular physical exercise has been shown to benefit neurocognitive functions, especially enhancing neurogenesis in the hippocampus. However, the effects of a single exercise session on cognitive functions are controversial. To address this issue, we measured hemodynamic changes in the brain during physical exercise using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) and investigated related effects on memory consolidation processes. Healthy young participants underwent two experimental visits. During each visit, they performed an associative memory task in which they first encoded a series of pictures, then spent 30-min exercising or resting, and finally were asked to recall the picture associations. We used NIRS to track changes in oxygenated hemoglobin concentration over the prefrontal cortex during exercise and rest. To characterize local tissue oxygenation and perfusion, we focused on low frequency oscillations in NIRS, also called vasomotion. We report a significant increase in associative memory consolidation after exercise, as compared to after rest, along with an overall increase in vasomotion. Additionally, performance improvement after exercise correlated positively with power in the neurogenic component (0.02 to 0.04 Hz) and negatively with power in the endothelial component (0.003 to 0.02 Hz). Overall, these results suggest that changes in vasomotion over the prefrontal cortex during exercise may promote memory consolidation processes.

1.

Introduction

Over the past decades, public policies have reiterated exercise recommendations, with a special emphasis placed on the benefits of physical activity on general health. Yet, regular physical exercise has also been shown to have a positive impact on cognition,1,2 notably in the memory domain.3

Experimental evidence in rodents has accumulated to support the claim that regular exercise improves brain plasticity and cognition by promoting neurogenesis and cell proliferation in the adult hippocampus,46 as well as dendritic length and complexity.7 In humans, it has been suggested that regular physical exercise enhances hippocampal and entorhinal gray matter density,8,9 and hence improves memory functions.10 But the effects of an acute bout of physical exercise on memory remain controversial, with some studies reporting positive effects11 and others reporting little or no effects,12 especially in meta-analyses.3,13,14 The brain mechanisms underlying the effects of exercise on memory processes is also a debated issue. For example, Basso et al.15 suggested that performance enhancement related to physical exercise is primarily linked to prefrontal and not hippocampal function, whereas a recent study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) after acute physical exercise revealed changes in hippocampal memory representations.11

Crucially, little is known about what is happening in the brain during physical exercise although this is when plasticity mechanisms should be modulated. Imaging brain activity using fMRI or electroencephalography (EEG) during exercise is problematic because of movement-related signal distortion. However, near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) is a method that has proved to be resistant to movement artifacts16 and may thus be an ideal tool to measure cerebral hemodynamic changes during physical exercise.

In addition to measuring standard hemodynamic changes, NIRS also allows measuring vasomotion-induced oscillations, which correspond to spontaneous low-frequency (0.003 to 0.15 Hz) rhythmic changes in the diameter of small vessels, see Aalkjaer et al.17 for review. Increased vasomotion was shown to lead to improved perfusion18,19 and local tissue oxygenation.20,21 Changes in vasomotor oscillations have a functional significance because they have been linked to cognitive load,22 and also to the onset of Alzheimer’s disease.23 Vasomotion can be divided into three frequency subbands, according to the origin of the oscillations.24 Low-frequency oscillations ranging from 0.04 to 0.15 Hz can be linked to the activity of smooth muscles of arterioles, thus called myogenic component.25 Lower frequency oscillations ranging from 0.02 to 0.04 Hz can be attributed to the intrinsic nervous activity and are referred to as neurogenic component.26 Ultralow oscillations between 0.003 and 0.02 Hz may reflect endothelial activity and mediated by the release of nitric oxide, henceforth named endothelial component.25,27

In this study, we used NIRS during an acute bout of medium intensity physical exercise or quiet rest to test whether exercise enhances memory consolidation processes and how such effects on memory may implicate changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) over the prefrontal cortex (the function of which may be improved by acute exercise15).

2.

Materials and Methods

2.1.

Participants

Seventeen healthy volunteers (10 females) between the ages of 21 and 39 (mean age of 29±5.7) participated in this study. All volunteers gave written informed consent and received financial compensation for their participation in this study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Geneva University Hospitals. All included participants were right-handed, nonsmokers, free from psychiatric, and neurological history reported performing aerobic exercise regularly (e.g., running, cycling, or swimming) at least twice a week and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The participants were requested to refrain from all caffeine and alcohol-containing beverages and intense physical activity for the 48 h preceding the experiment. All participants first came for an introductory session to get acquainted with the task, and then underwent two experimental sessions, according to a randomized, crossover design (each visit was separated by at least 3 days). Three participants were excluded from the analyses: one participant was performing at chance level in the memory task and for two participants, NIRS data were not exploitable.

2.2.

Experimental Procedure

We adapted an associative memory task28 consisting of two parts: training (subdivided into encoding and learning) and test, separated by an exercise or rest period [Fig. 1(a)]. For each experimental visit, the participants first performed the encoding and learning parts of the memory task. Then, we placed the NIRS optodes over their prefrontal cortex [Figs. 1(d) and 1(e)] before they exercised (on a cycle ergometer) or rested for 30 min. After 10 min of recovery, during which their vigilance level was assessed [psychomotor vigilance task (PVT)],29 the participants performed the test part of the memory task. During the introductory session, all participants performed a shortened version of the associative task (encoding, learning, and test).

Fig. 1

Procedure. (a) Experimental design. All participants underwent two experimental visits composed of an encoding and learning sessions of the associative memory task followed by either 30 min of medium intensity exercise or 30 of quiet rest. Then all participants performed a PVT task and the test part of the associative memory task. (b) Associative memory task: example of one series of the office and the shoe theme. Themes were counterbalanced between first and second visits and the exercise and rest conditions. (c) Test part of the associative memory task, example of direct, inference 1, and inference 2 trials. (d) Schematic representation of the optode template used for NIRS measurements, solid lines show measured channels, the dashed line shows an excluded channel as it is crossing a main facial vessel. Interoptode distance is 35 mm except for short channel at 15 mm. Fpz was localized according to standard 10-20 EEG system. (e) Example of participant during the exercise condition wearing NIRS.

NPH_4_4_041404_f001.png

To avoid interference and learning across visits, we designed three versions of the memory task, one shortened version for the introductory session, and two full versions for the experimental visits. The experimental visits’ versions were matched in difficulty and counterbalanced across the exercise condition (exercise and rest) and the visits (first and second) [see Fig. 1(b)]. During each experimental visit, the participants had to memorize eight different series containing six pictures belonging to a specific theme: “office” or “shoe shop” (one theme per visit). For the introductory session, the participants had to memorize five series of a “swimming pool” theme.

2.2.1.

Encoding

The participants were shown each of the eight series of pictures once, one picture at a time (2000 ms per picture) and were asked to memorize each sequence as a whole.

2.2.2.

Learning

For each series, the first picture was shown [pen, for the “office” theme in this example; Fig. 1(b)] followed by the same picture with two options for the second picture in the series (chair), one being the correct next picture and the other picture being from a different series. The participants had to select the correct next picture by pressing a right or left button. The correct picture was then shown (providing a feedback for each trial), followed by this same picture together with the two next options for the third picture in the series (desk). This continued until the last picture in the series (office building). Learning comprised three blocks, each with all eight series shown once.

2.2.3.

Exercise and rest conditions

For the exercise condition, the participants pedaled on a cycle ergometer (Ergoline GmbH, Bitz, Germany) at a 50-W power. The pedaling frequency was kept between 60 and 80 cycles per minute, which was shown on a small screen in front of the participant. For the rest condition, the participants were to sit on a chair and were allowed to quietly read magazines. Both exercise and rest conditions lasted for 30 min. The participants were wearing a Polar RS800CX N device to measure heart rate throughout both experimental visits. Since the experimental exercise intensity was identical for all participants, we could derive estimates of individual fitness levels from heart rate.30 This was done by first calculating the theoretical maximal heart rate for each participant based on the age adjusted maximal heart rate (AAMHR) formula.31 Based on the assumption that a higher fitness level results in a lowered heart rate (for a given exercise intensity—50 W here—and young participants), we computed the percentage of AAMHR to which each participant’s mean heart rate corresponded. This measure provided an estimate of individual fitness level.

2.2.4.

Test

The participants’ memory for the series of pictures was assessed. The participants were presented with one cue picture and two other pictures, among which they had to select the one belonging to the same series as the cue picture. The two options could represent the immediate next item in the series (direct trials) or could be separated by one or two items from the cue picture [inference of order 1 or order 2 trials; Fig. 1(c)]. All types of trials were shown in a randomized order and were presented in the same format as during learning, with the exception that no feedback was shown.

2.3.

NIRS Measurements

2.3.1.

Data acquisition

For this study, we used the Oxymon MKIII device (Artinis Medical Systems B.V., Elst, the Netherlands) with eight optodes combined in an overlapping fashion to form six channels [see full lines on Fig. 1(d)]. Five out of six channels were placed at an interoptode distance of 35 mm, and one channel was set at 15 mm (short separation channel) to subsequently remove effects of sudation and capillary blood flow increase in the superficial layers of the head32 (scalp and skull, see Sec. 2.3.2). For all the NIRS measurements, the used interoptode distances and optode placement was consistent with the literature for recording of brain hemodynamics during aerobic exercise.33,34

The optode holder was placed on the forehead according to the EEG 10-20 system, so that Fpz was at the exact center of the square formed by the holder [Fig. 1(d)]. Having a fixed optode-holder, the rest of the optodes fell into place as described above. These positions allowed us to record blood oxygenation level-dependent activity over the medial prefrontal cortex but avoiding main facial vessels [avoiding the supraorbital vein and discarding the channel that goes over the frontal branch of the superficial temporal vessels, see dashed line Fig. 1(d)]. The sampling rate was 10 Hz, corresponding to the literature standards35,36 and measurements were biased to zero at the beginning of both the exercise and the rest periods to track only hemoglobin level changes.

2.3.2.

NIRS analysis

Data were preprocessed using standard procedures as implemented in Homer2 (MGH-Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts).37 Motion artifacts were defined as signal change of 0.1 or higher in optical density units over 0.5 s. These artifacts, plus the 0.5 s before and after them, were removed from the analysis. We then applied a low-pass filter of 0.5 Hz to remove the heart beat and respiration effects. We performed a short channel separation correction to remove any effects from scalp and skull, such as sudation and superficial vasodilation from the five deeper channels. This correction was done using a third-order polynomial regressor to extract the signal coming from the superficial layers of the head (picked up by the short separation channel) from the rest of the channels.38 All analyses were performed on the HbO2 and HHb signals. Because it is widely accepted that spontaneous oscillations are more prominent in HbO2 than in HHb data,24,3941 we focused our analyses on HbO2 oscillations (see Appendix for HHb results).

Data from the first and last 2.5 min of the exercise and rest periods were excluded to minimize unspecific modulations of signal at the start and end of the pedaling. Mean signal and standard deviation (SD) were extracted. Power spectral density (PSD) was calculated using Welch’s method for the interval between 0.003 and 0.15 Hz, i.e., in the frequency range of vasomotion.17 Vasomotion was further subdivided into three sub-bands or components corresponding to different oscillatory sources: endothelial (0.003 to 0.02 Hz), neurogenic (0.02 to 0.04 Hz), and myogenic (0.04 to 0.15 Hz).41 In order to make valid comparisons of SDs and PSDs of different vasomotion components between the exercise and rest conditions, the SDs and PSDs of HbO2 signal were normalized to their mean values across exercise and rest periods:24,39,40

normSDexercise=2×SDexercise/(SDexercise+SDrest),normSDrest=2×SDrest/(SDexercise+SDrest).

For the PSDs, the normalization was similar, but calculated for endothelial, neurogenic, and myogenic components separately.

normPSDexercise=2×PSDexercise/(PSDexercise+PSDrest),normPSDrest=2×PSDrest/(PSDexercise+PSDrest).

To be able to compare the different components to each other, normalization was performed relative to the mean of the three components as follows:

normPSDendothelial=3×PSDendothelial/(PSDendothelial+PSDneurogenic+PSDmyogenic),normPSDneurogenic=3×PSDneurogenic/(PSDendothelial+PSDneurogenic+PSDmyogenic),normPSDmyogenic=3×PSDmyogenic/(PSDendothelial+PSDneurogenic+PSDmyogenic).

For all the analyses of variance (ANOVA), we included a channel factor as repeated measure to assess if there was an effect of the different channels on normalized SD and PSD data. We found no main effect of channel and no interaction effect with channel (all p>0.05), so channels were pooled for the subsequent analyses. We performed pairwise t-tests to assess difference between conditions (exercise and rest), and one-way repeated measures ANOVA with components of vasomotion as factor to test for differences between endothelial, neurogenic, and myogenic frequency bands (followed by Bonferroni correction of post hoc pairwise comparisons). Correlations with performance were done using Spearman’s correlation on ranks as normal distribution and equal variance criteria were not met in our sample.

2.4.

Memory Performance Analysis

For the analysis of the associative memory task, a learning criterion was applied so that, during the test phase, we only included the responses to those pictures that had actually been learnt by the participants. We considered that a picture was learnt when during the learning session, the participant correctly chose it at least twice across the three learning blocks. During the test part only the trials where these pictures were the picture to be chosen were included in the analysis. Using this criterion, on average 3.1±2.26 pictures per participant were excluded. All behavioral analyses were performed on accuracy data (percentage of correct responses) and on reaction time data (for correct responses) using Statistica (Version 13, StatSoft, Inc. TULSA, Oklahoma).

3.

Results

3.1.

Behavioral Results

3.1.1.

Learning

Accuracy and reaction time data were analyzed using two repeated measures ANOVA with learning blocks as factor (block 1, block 2, and block 3). Accuracy analysis revealed a main effect of block [F(2,26)=10.730, p=0.0004] consistent with a progressive learning of the associations. During the third block, the participants reached a high level of performance (79.27±6.78% of correct responses). Importantly, there was no effect of “experimental visit number” [F(1,13)=0.244, p=0.629] or “visit theme” [F(1,13)=0.334, p=0.572] when those factors were added as repeated measures to the previous ANOVA. There was no difference in reaction times across blocks [F(2,26)=1.924, p=0.166].

3.1.2.

Test

Accuracy and reaction time data from the test were analyzed using two repeated-measures ANOVAs with condition (exercise and rest) and inferences (direct, inference 1, and inference 2) as repeated measures. For accuracy, we found a significant effect of condition on memory performance [F(1,13)=17.272, p=0.001], but no effect of inference [F(2,26)=3.011, p=0.065; Fig. 2(a)] indicating that the participants perform better after exercise than after rest. For reaction times, there was no effect of condition [F(1,13)=2.640, p=0.128] but an effect of inference [F(2,26)=6.394, p=0.006). Post-hoc analysis showed that the participants perform inference 1 and inference 2 trials slower than direct trials (pdirect-inf1=0.012, pdirect-inf2=0.016).

Fig. 2

Behavioral and NIRS results. (a) Improved associative memory performance at test after exercise than after rest. (b) Group power spectrum density plot from cerebral HbO2. (c) Normalized SD of the HbO2 signal is significantly higher during exercise than during rest. (d) Relative mean normalized PSDs between conditions (exercise and rest). (e) Relative mean normalized PSDs within condition.

NPH_4_4_041404_f002.png

3.1.3.

Psychomotor vigilance task

We report no difference in PVT data after exercise or after rest (paired t-tests, all p-values >0.05, see Table 1), indicating that the participants were equally vigilant after exercise and after rest.

Table 1

Comparison of PVT results after exercise and after rest.

PVT after exercisePVT after restt-test p-value
Mean reaction times312.238311.0230.907
Median reaction times295.941296.1180.982
Standard error of RT9.53310.6930.652
Number of false alarms0.1180.1760.641
Number of lapses1.2351.2940.911

3.2.

Heart Rate

Heart rate was 69.3±9.9 at rest and 111.6±17.8 during exercise [t(26)=8.565, p<0.001]. The heart rate observed during exercise corresponded to 58.8±8.5% of participants’ theoretical maximal heart rate. There was no difference in heart rate during the encoding and test sessions, both of which were separated by the exercise or the rest condition [paired t-test texercise(26)=0.369, p=0.715; trest(26)=1.457, p=0.157], and there was no correlation between exercise heart rate and performance on the task (R=0.0605, p=0.852) or with power in the vasomotion frequency band (R=0.0649, p=0.841), neither did percentage of AAMHR correlate with these measures (with performance on the task: R=0.0623, p=0.848; with power in the vasomotion frequency band: R=1.696, p=0.618).

3.3.

Body Mass Index and Gender

All participants had a BMI between 18 and 25, except for one who was at 26. There was no significant correlation between BMI and task performance improvement (R=0.267, p=0.378) or power in the vasomotion frequency band (Rexercise=0.087, p=0.778; Rrest=0.093, p=0.762).

We ran one-way repeated measures ANOVAs with gender as a factor for all of our measures and found no effect of gender for any of them (all p>0.1).

3.4.

NIRS

3.4.1.

Exercise versus rest

Normalized SD of the HbO2 signal was significantly higher during exercise than during rest [t(26)=2.566, p=0.016; Fig. 2(c)] while the mean HbO2 signal was not significantly different [t(26)=0.036, p=0.971]. This indicates that the HbO2 signal showed significantly higher amplitude fluctuations during the exercise condition. Power spectrum was extracted for every participant to look at the vasomotion-induced oscillations in the frequency range between 0.003 and 0.15 Hz. Figure 2(b) shows the mean power spectrum density for all participants. As shown in Fig. 2(d), the power of all three vasomotion components increased significantly during exercise: endothelial [t(26)=2.086, p=0.047], neurogenic [t(26)=2.957, p=0.007], and myogenic [t(26)=5.331, p<0.001].

3.4.2.

Within condition

One-way repeated measures ANOVAs were run for each condition (exercise and rest) separately and with the component of vasomotion as a factor. This yielded a significant effect of component for each condition [exercise: F(2,26)=380.27, p<0.001; rest: F(2,26)=347.45, p<0.001). Bonferroni post hoc analysis revealed a predominant effect of the endothelial component in vasomotion, followed by the neurogenic component, and the myogenic component (post hoc effects: endothelial-neurogenicexercise: p<0.001, endothelial-myogenicexercise: p<0.001, neurogenic-myogenicexercise: p<0.001; endothelial-neurogenicrest: p<0.001, endothelial-myogenicrest: p<0.001, neurogenic-myogenicrest: p<0.001) [Fig. 2(e)].

To test for a link between vasomotion and memory consolidation, we performed correlations between each component of vasomotion (normalized within condition) during physical exercise and exercise-related performance improvement (performanceexercise-performancerest). We observed a significantly positive relationship between the neurogenic component and performance improvement [R=0.547, p=0.043; Fig. 3(a)], and a significantly negative correlation between the endothelial component and performance improvement [R=0.591, p=0.026; Fig. 3(b)]. No correlation was found with the myogenic component [R=0.243, p=0.401; Fig. 3(c)].

Fig. 3

Correlation between memory consolidation and HbO2 vasomotion during exercise. (a) Performance enhancement due to exercise correlated positively with PSD in the neurogenic component. (b) Performance enhancement due to exercise correlated negatively with PSD in the endothelial component. (c) Performance enhancement due to exercise did not correlate with PSD in the myogenic component.

NPH_4_4_041404_f003.png

4.

Discussion

In this study, we show (1) that 30 min of medium intense exercise enhances the consolidation of associative memories; (2) that the SD of the HbO2 signal and the power over all three components of vasomotion (endothelial, neurogenic, and myogenic) are higher during exercise as compared to rest; and (3) that memory performance increase due to exercise correlates positively with the neurogenic vasomotion component and negatively with the endothelial component. Overall these results suggest that modulations of vasomotor activity over the prefrontal cortex during exercise are associated with enhanced memory consolidation.

4.1.

Better Memory Performance After Exercise

The increase in performance after exercise as compared to after rest is in line with recent results by Van Dongen et al.11 showing that high intensity exercise has a positive effect on memory consolidation and memory coding in the hippocampus (tested 48 h after exercise) but only if exercising occurred 4 h after initial learning. Our results demonstrate that memory effects may also be seen for medium intensity exercise, and when the latter takes place immediately after learning. Previous research on exercise has also highlighted the effect of acute exercise on executive functions,15,35,42 and we know that memory performance may be enhanced by increased attention and vigilance.43 In our study, we did not find any difference in PVT measures after exercise from after rest (Table 1), suggesting that the observed beneficial effect of exercise on memory was not primarily driven enhanced attention or vigilance. We also sought to assess whether participant’s individual fitness level influenced their performance. Since exercise level was constant for all participants in our study, we estimated fitness levels from the percentage of AAMHR heart rate measures (detailed in Sec. 2.2.3). We found no correlations with performance, which overall suggests that it is unlikely that individual fitness level is a major contributor to the memory consolidation enhancement we report here.

4.2.

Increased Vasomotion During Exercise

For the NIRS measurements, a short separation channel was included to record extracranial signal and correct for sudation-related artifacts and cutaneous venous contribution including systemic scalp effects.4447 Although cerebral systemic effects may differ from scalp systemic effects, it is acknowledged that slow oscillations in the vasomotion frequency bands, that we analyzed here, are less contaminated by systemic effects as for example Mayer waves (0.1 Hz) or hemodynamic response functions.38 It has been suggested that the short channel location has an important role, as superficial artifacts are not homogeneously distributed, but are rather localized in the scalp draining veins.48 Ideally, we should have included as many short channels as regular channels in our design,49 but this was unfortunately not possible with our equipment. However, we carefully positioned all our channels so that they did not cross main arteries or veins. Moreover, we reported no effect of channel for any of our HbO2 measures, which further supports that our optode placement and short channel correction was efficient.

When comparing exercise and rest, we found no difference in the overall mean of HbO2 signal, consistent with previous findings demonstrating constant global brain blood flow during low-to-moderate exercise in healthy humans,50 see meta-analysis in Ref. 51. By contrast, we reported increased power for all three vasomotion components during the exercise condition (compared to rest). This increase in power is most likely linked to vasodilation52 and, hence, to increased oxygenation, which is largely supported by previous findings.16,51,53,54 We also found that, in both conditions, the endothelial component was predominant, followed by the neurogenic component, and by the myogenic component [Fig. 2(d)]. This general profile is reminiscent of what was previously reported for light sleep, slow wave sleep, and REM in healthy subjects.24 It has been suggested that the endothelial frequency range is the most effective for cerebral autoregulation,55 which may explain why endothelial component is predominant across different physiological states, including exercise.

4.3.

Relating Vasomotor Activity to Memory Consolidation

This is the first time to our knowledge that vasomotor activity during physical exercise has been linked to improvement in memory. Here, we present evidence that 30 min of medium intensity exercise enhances associative memory consolidation and that this effect likely relates to changes in vasomotion especially in the neurogenic and endothelial components over the prefrontal cortex. Although relatively little is known about the effects of vasomotion on cognition, a recent finding suggests that vasomotion is impaired in Alzheimer’s disease.23 On one hand, moderate physical exercise is known to increase glucose uptake in the brain,56 on the other hand, power in the neurogenic component and glucose uptake have been directly linked.57 Based on these observations, we would like to propose that, in our study, higher neurogenic activity during exercise may be associated with increased glucose uptake levels over prefrontal cortices, which in turn would boost memory consolidation processes.58

More generally, we show that near-infrared spectroscopy may be a valuable method for investigating brain plasticity during physical exercise. Finally, our results may challenge and refine models of memory consolidation that generally focus on rest or sleep periods while we show that an active period of physical exercise may yield similar improvements in memory.

Appendices

Appendix A:

HHb Results

A.1.

Analysis of HHb Signal between Conditions

Normalized SD of the HHb signal was significantly higher during exercise than during rest [t(26)=2.965, p=0.006, see Fig. 4(b)] while the mean HHb signal was not significantly different between conditions [t(26)=0.537, p=0.596]. This indicates significantly higher amplitude in the HHb signal during the exercise condition.

Fig. 4

HHb NIRS results. (a) Group power spectrum density plot from cerebral HHb (b) Normalized SD of the HHb signal was significantly higher during exercise than during rest. (c) Relative mean normalized PSDs between conditions (exercise and rest). (d) Relative mean normalized PSDs within condition.

NPH_4_4_041404_f004.png

Power spectrum was extracted for every subject to assess the vasomotion-induced oscillations in the frequency range of 0.003 to 0.15 Hz. Figure 4(a) shows the mean power spectrum density for all subjects. As shown in Fig. 4(c), the power of the neurogenic and myogenic components increased significantly during exercise {neurogenic [t(26)=3.173, p=0.004], myogenic [t(26)=4.682, p<0.001]}, although power in the endothelial component did not differ {endothelial [t(26)=0.564, p=0.577]}.

A.2.

Analysis of HHb Signal within Conditions

After normalization across exercise and rest condition, we report an effect of component for both conditions [exercise condition: F(2,26)=38.498, p<0.001; rest condition: F(2,26)=721.83, p<0.001, Fig. 4(d)]. Bonferroni post hoc analysis shows a predominant effect of the endothelial component in vasomotion (post hoc effects: endothelial-neurogenic: p<0.001, endothelial-myogenic: p<0.001). The neurogenic component was significantly higher than the myogenic component for the rest condition but only trended toward significance in the exercise condition (neurogenic-myogenic: prest<0.001, pexercise=0.073).

No significant correlations were found between task performance and any of the components of vasomotion (endothelial: R=0.225, p=0.459; neurogenic: R=0.214, p=0.482; myogenic: R=0.165, p=0.590).

Disclosures

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Brain and Behavior Laboratory of the University of Geneva (Geneva, Switzerland) for providing help and imaging facilities. We thank Anna Custo for her help with NIRS data analysis and Katalin Bartha for useful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (No 320030_135653 to S. Schwartz, 32003B_127620 and 3200B0-114033 to G. Ferretti), the National Center of Competence in Research (NCCR) Affective Sciences financed by the Swiss National Science Foundation (No 51NF40-104897 to S. Schwartz), K.I. was supported by the AXA foundation. B.M.B., A.B., G.F., S.S., and K.I. designed the experiments. B.M.B., A.B., and K.I. acquired the data. B.M.B, A.B., S.S., and K.I. analyzed the data. B.M.B., A.B., G.F., S.S., and K.I. wrote the manuscript.

References

1. 

S. Colcombe and A. F. Kramer, “Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study,” Psychol. Sci., 14 (2), 125 –130 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psci.2003.14.issue-2 1467-9280 Google Scholar

2. 

P. J. Smith et al., “Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials,” Psychosom. Med., 72 (3), 239 –252 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633 Google Scholar

3. 

M. Roig et al., “The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: a review with meta-analysis,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 37 (8), 1645 –1666 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012 NBREDE 0149-7634 Google Scholar

4. 

A. C. Pereira et al., “An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 104 (13), 5638 –5643 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0611721104 Google Scholar

5. 

N. Uysal et al., “The effects of regular aerobic exercise in adolescent period on hippocampal neuron density, apoptosis and spatial memory,” Neurosci. Lett., 383 (3), 241 –245 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2005.04.054 Google Scholar

6. 

C. W. Wu et al., “Treadmill exercise counteracts the suppressive effects of peripheral lipopolysaccharide on hippocampal neurogenesis and learning and memory,” J. Neurochem., 103 (6), 2471 –2481 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04987.x Google Scholar

7. 

A. M. Stranahan, K. Lee and M. P. Mattson, “Central mechanisms of HPA axis regulation by voluntary exercise,” Neuromol. Med., 10 (2), 118 –127 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12017-008-8027-0 Google Scholar

8. 

K. I. Erickson et al., “Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 108 (7), 3017 –3022 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015950108 Google Scholar

9. 

A. S. Whiteman et al., “Entorhinal volume, aerobic fitness, and recognition memory in healthy young adults: a voxel-based morphometry study,” Neuroimage, 126 229 –238 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.049 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

10. 

M. W. Voss et al., “Bridging animal and human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity,” Trends Cognit. Sci., 17 (10), 525 –544 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.001 Google Scholar

11. 

E. V. van Dongen et al., “Physical exercise performed four hours after learning improves memory retention and increases hippocampal pattern similarity during retrieval,” Curr. Biol., 26 (13), 1722 –1727 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.071 Google Scholar

12. 

K. Hotting et al., “The effects of acute physical exercise on memory, peripheral BDNF, and cortisol in young adults,” Neural Plast., 2016 6860573 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/6860573 Google Scholar

13. 

Y. K. Chang et al., “The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis,” Brain Res., 1453 87 –101 (2012). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068 Google Scholar

14. 

P. D. Tomporowski, “Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition,” Acta Psychol., 112 (3), 297 –324 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8 Google Scholar

15. 

J. C. Basso et al., “Acute exercise improves prefrontal cortex but not hippocampal function in healthy adults,” J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 21 (10), 791 –801 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S135561771500106X Google Scholar

16. 

T. Rupp and S. Perrey, “Prefrontal cortex oxygenation and neuromuscular responses to exhaustive exercise,” Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 102 (2), 153 –163 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00421-007-0568-7 EJAPFN 1439-6319 Google Scholar

17. 

C. Aalkjaer, D. Boedtkjer and V. Matchkov, “Vasomotion—what is currently thought?,” Acta Physiol., 202 (3), 253 –269 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apha.2011.202.issue-3 APACAB 0001-6756 Google Scholar

18. 

T. Sakurai and N. Terui, “Effects of sympathetically induced vasomotion on tissue-capillary fluid exchange,” Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol., 291 (4), H1761 –H1767 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/ajpheart.00280.2006 Google Scholar

19. 

M. Rucker et al., “Vasomotion in critically perfused muscle protects adjacent tissues from capillary perfusion failure,” Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol., 279 (2), H550 –558 (2000). Google Scholar

20. 

H. Nilsson and C. Aalkjaer, “Vasomotion: mechanisms and physiological importance,” Mol. Interventions, 3 (2), 79 –89 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1124/mi.3.2.79 MIONAR 1534-0384 Google Scholar

21. 

A. G. Tsai and M. Intaglietta, “Evidence of flowmotion induced changes in local tissue oxygenation,” Int. J. Microcirc. Clin. Exp., 12 (1), 75 –88 (1993). IMCEDT 0167-6865 Google Scholar

22. 

A. Vermeij et al., “Very-low-frequency oscillations of cerebral hemodynamics and blood pressure are affected by aging and cognitive load,” Neuroimage, 85 (Pt 1), 608 –615 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.107 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

23. 

L. Y. Di Marco et al., “Is vasomotion in cerebral arteries impaired in Alzheimer’s disease?,” J. Alzheimer’s Dis., 46 (1), 35 –53 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/JAD-142976 Google Scholar

24. 

Z. Zhang and R. Khatami, “Predominant endothelial vasomotor activity during human sleep: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,” Eur. J. Neurosci., 40 (9), 3396 –3404 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12702 Google Scholar

25. 

A. Stefanovska, “Coupled oscillators. Complex but not complicated cardiovascular and brain interactions,” IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag., 26 (6), 25 –29 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/EMB.2007.907088 Google Scholar

26. 

R. Zhang et al., “Autonomic neural control of dynamic cerebral autoregulation in humans,” Circulation, 106 (14), 1814 –1820 (2002). http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.0000031798.07790.FE CIRCAZ 0009-7322 Google Scholar

27. 

H. D. Kvernmo et al., “Oscillations in the human cutaneous blood perfusion signal modified by endothelium-dependent and endothelium-independent vasodilators,” Microvasc. Res., 57 (3), 298 –309 (1999). http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/mvre.1998.2139 MIVRA6 0026-2862 Google Scholar

28. 

K. Igloi et al., “A nap to recap or how reward regulates hippocampal-prefrontal memory networks during daytime sleep in humans,” eLife, 4 e07903 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.07903 Google Scholar

29. 

D. F. Dinges et al., “Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night,” Sleep, 20 (4), 267 –277 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/20.4.267 SLEED6 0161-8105 Google Scholar

30. 

J. S. Skinner et al., “Heart rate versus %VO2max: age, sex, race, initial fitness, and training response–HERITAGE,” Med. Sci. Sports Exercise, 35 (11), 1908 –1913 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000093607.57995.E3 Google Scholar

31. 

H. Tanaka, K. D. Monahan and D. R. Seals, “Age-predicted maximal heart rate revisited,” J. Am. Coll. Cardiol., 37 (1), 153 –156 (2001). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0735-1097(00)01054-8 Google Scholar

32. 

S. Tak and J. C. Ye, “Statistical analysis of fNIRS data: a comprehensive review,” NeuroImage, 85 (Pt 1), 72 –91 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.016 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

33. 

F. Billaut et al., “Cerebral oxygenation decreases but does not impair performance during self-paced, strenuous exercise,” Acta Physiol., 198 (4), 477 –486 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/aps.2010.198.issue-4 APACAB 0001-6756 Google Scholar

34. 

T. C. Fu et al., “Aerobic interval training improves oxygen uptake efficiency by enhancing cerebral and muscular hemodynamics in patients with heart failure,” Int. J. Cardiol., 167 (1), 41 –50 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcard.2011.11.086 IJCDD5 0167-5273 Google Scholar

35. 

H. Yanagisawa et al., “Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test,” NeuroImage, 50 (4), 1702 –1710 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.023 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

36. 

T. Takahashi et al., “Influence of skin blood flow on near-infrared spectroscopy signals measured on the forehead during a verbal fluency task,” NeuroImage, 57 (3), 991 –1002 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.012 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

37. 

T. J. Huppert et al., “HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain,” Appl. Opt., 48 (10), D280 –298 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/AO.48.00D280 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

38. 

M. A. Yucel et al., “Mayer waves reduce the accuracy of estimated hemodynamic response functions in functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Biomed. Opt. Express, 7 (8), 3078 –3088 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/BOE.7.003078 BOEICL 2156-7085 Google Scholar

39. 

C. Kolyva et al., “Oscillations in cerebral haemodynamics in patients with falciparum malaria,” in Oxygen Transport to Tissue XXXIV, 101 –107 (2013). Google Scholar

40. 

M. L. Schroeter, O. Schmiedel and D. Y. von Cramon, “Spontaneous low-frequency oscillations decline in the aging brain,” J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 24 (10), 1183 –1191 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.WCB.0000135231.90164.40 Google Scholar

41. 

H. Obrig et al., “Spontaneous low frequency oscillations of cerebral hemodynamics and metabolism in human adults,” NeuroImage, 12 (6), 623 –639 (2000). http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0657 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

42. 

L. Johnson et al., “An acute bout of exercise improves the cognitive performance of older adults,” J. Aging Phys. Act., 24 (4), 591 –598 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/japa.2015-0097 Google Scholar

43. 

C. Schmidt et al., “A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition,” Cognit. Neuropsychol., 24 (7), 755 –789 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158 COGNEP 1464-0627 Google Scholar

44. 

J. Choi et al., “Noninvasive determination of the optical properties of adult brain: near-infrared spectroscopy approach,” J. Biomed. Opt., 9 (1), 221 –229 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.1628242 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

45. 

R. B. Saager and A. J. Berger, “Direct characterization and removal of interfering absorption trends in two-layer turbid media,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A, 22 (9), 1874 –1882 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.22.001874 Google Scholar

46. 

R. B. Saager, N. L. Telleri and A. J. Berger, “Two-detector corrected near infrared spectroscopy (C-NIRS) detects hemodynamic activation responses more robustly than single-detector NIRS,” Neuroimage, 55 (4), 1679 –1685 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.043 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

47. 

E. Kirilina et al., “The physiological origin of task-evoked systemic artefacts in functional near infrared spectroscopy,” NeuroImage, 61 (1), 70 –81 (2012). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.074 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

48. 

L. Gagnon et al., “Short separation channel location impacts the performance of short channel regression in NIRS,” Neuroimage, 59 (3), 2518 –2528 (2012). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.095 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

49. 

M. A. Yucel et al., “Short separation regression improves statistical significance and better localizes the hemodynamic response obtained by near-infrared spectroscopy for tasks with differing autonomic responses,” Neurophotonics, 2 (3), 035005 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.2.3.035005 Google Scholar

50. 

K. Ide and N. H. Secher, “Cerebral blood flow and metabolism during exercise,” Prog. Neurobiol., 61 (4), 397 –414 (2000). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(99)00057-X PGNBA5 0301-0082 Google Scholar

51. 

C. R. Rooks et al., “Effects of incremental exercise on cerebral oxygenation measured by near-infrared spectroscopy: a systematic review,” Prog. Neurobiol., 92 (2), 134 –150 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.002 PGNBA5 0301-0082 Google Scholar

52. 

Z. Zhang and R. Khatami, “A biphasic change of regional blood volume in the frontal cortex during non-rapid eye movement sleep: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,” Sleep, 38 (8), 1211 –1217 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.5665/sleep.4894 SLEED6 0161-8105 Google Scholar

53. 

C. S. Bediz et al., “Acute supramaximal exercise increases the brain oxygenation in relation to cognitive workload,” Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 174 (2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00174 Google Scholar

54. 

S. Perrey, “Non-invasive NIR spectroscopy of human brain function during exercise,” Methods, 45 (4), 289 –299 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ymeth.2008.04.005 MTHDE9 1046-2023 Google Scholar

55. 

R. Zhang, J. H. Zuckerman and B. D. Levine, “Spontaneous fluctuations in cerebral blood flow: insights from extended-duration recordings in humans,” Am. J Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol., 278 (6), H1848 –1855 (2000). Google Scholar

56. 

M. K. Dalsgaard, “Fuelling cerebral activity in exercising man,” J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 26 (6), 731 –750 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600256 Google Scholar

57. 

M. P. de Boer et al., “Insulin-induced changes in microvascular vasomotion and capillary recruitment are associated in humans,” Microcirculation, 21 (5), 380 –387 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/micc.2014.21.issue-5 MCCRD8 0275-4177 Google Scholar

58. 

S. Hoyer, “Memory function and brain glucose metabolism,” Pharmacopsychiatry, 36 (Suppl. 1), S62 –S67 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2003-40452 PHRMEZ 0176-3679 Google Scholar

Biography

Blanca Marin Bosch received her BS degree in pharmacy from the University of Valencia, Spain, in 2012, and her MSc degree in neuroscience from the University of Geneva, Switzerland, in 2015. Currently, she is a PhD student at the Department of Fundamental Neurosciences at the University of Geneva, Switzerland. Her interests include functional near-infrared spectroscopy, associative memory, and physical exercise.

Aurélien Bringard has a PhD in exercise science and is currently a research assistant at the University of Geneva. His research interests include pulmonary oxygen uptake and cardiovascular regulation at exercise onset, control of baroreflex, and blood pressure regulation.

Guido Ferretti has an MD/PhD degree from the University of Milan, Italy and is currently an associate professor at the University of Geneva.

Sophie Schwartz is an associate professor at the University of Geneva. Her research interests include perceptual and emotional learning, neural plasticity, sleep, functional MRI, electroencephalography, and polysomnography.

Kinga Iglói received her MSc degree in biology and social sciences from the Ecole Normale Supérieure, Paris, France. She received her PhD from the LPPA (College de France) Paris, France, and did part of her PhD at the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience (ICN UCL) in London, United Kingdom. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow at the University of Geneva, Switzerland. Her main research interests are hippocampus dependent memory processes and the influence of physical exercise on these processes.

CC BY: © The Authors. Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI.
Blanca Marin Bosch, Aurélien Bringard, Giulio Ferretti, Sophie Schwartz, and Kinga Iglói "Effect of cerebral vasomotion during physical exercise on associative memory, a near-infrared spectroscopy study," Neurophotonics 4(4), 041404 (25 July 2017). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.041404
Received: 1 April 2017; Accepted: 5 July 2017; Published: 25 July 2017
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 24 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
Back to Top