Open Access
24 July 2021 Investigating developmental changes in scalp-to-cortex correspondence using diffuse optical tomography sensitivity in infancy
Xiaoxue Fu, John E. Richards
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Significance: Diffuse optical tomography (DOT) uses near-infrared light spectroscopy (NIRS) to measure changes in cerebral hemoglobin concentration. Anatomical interpretations of NIRS data require accurate descriptions of the cranio-cerebral relations and DOT sensitivity to the underlying cortical structures. Such information is limited for pediatric populations because they undergo rapid head and brain development.

Aim: We aim to investigate age-related differences in scalp-to-cortex distance and mapping between scalp locations and cortical regions of interest (ROIs) among infants (2 weeks to 24 months with narrow age bins), children (4 and 12 years), and adults (20 to 24 years).

Approach: We used spatial scalp projection and photon propagation simulation methods with age-matched realistic head models based on MRIs.

Results: There were age-group differences in the scalp-to-cortex distances in infancy. The developmental increase was magnified in children and adults. There were systematic age-related differences in the probabilistic mappings between scalp locations and cortical ROIs.

Conclusions: Our findings have important implications in the design of sensor placement and making anatomical interpretations in NIRS and fNIRS research. Age-appropriate, realistic head models should be used to provide anatomical guidance for standalone DOT data in infants.

1.

Introduction

Diffuse optical tomography (DOT) uses near-infrared light spectroscopy (NIRS) to measure changes in cerebral hemoglobin concentration.13 DOT does not provide anatomical information about the location of the hemodynamic signal. Spatial scalp projection can be implemented to interrogate the brain region(s) underlying the scalp sensor (i.e., optodes) locations.4 An alternative approach is to generate a forward model of DOT sensitivity using photon propagation simulations.5 The forward model can then guide DOT image reconstruction to recover the brain locations of hemoglobin concentration changes.6,7 There are considerable brain structural changes during infancy through childhood and adulthood.8,9 Accurate models of DOT sensitivity must account for age-related changes in the head.10

1.1.

Scalp-to-Cortex Distances Measured by Scalp Projection

The initial step to understanding the underlying cortical structures being measured with NIRS is to measure the distance from the NIRS recording locations to the cortical surface. The 10–20, 10–10, and 10–5 systems provide a standardized and reproducible method for NIRS optode placement.11 Spatial scalp projection uses an algorithm to project a standard electrode location on the scalp down to a location on the cortical surface.4,12 Hence, it maps the scalp-to-cortex correspondence based on spatial locations. Adult studies projected 10–2013 and 10–10 electrodes14 to standard brain templates. Okamoto et al.13 found that the scalp-to-cortex distance was shallower in the frontal, temporal, and occipital regions but deeper in the parietal and along the intrahemispherical fissure. Scalp projection facilitates the understanding of the sampling depth required to measure the cortical activities using NIRS recording or DOT.

It is important to examine age-related changes in scalp-to-cortex distance. There is extensive brain morphological development from infancy to adolescence.8,9,15,16 One change is the distance from the scalp to cortical landmarks [Heschl’s gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, frontal pole, occipital pole, parieto-occipital sulcus, and vertex (Vz)]. There are significant increases in scalp-to-cortex distances from newborn to age 12.17 Kabdebon et al.18 virtually placed 10–20 electrodes on individual MRIs from 3- to 17-week-old infants. The distance between the scalp electrode positions and the cortical surface decreased from the frontal to occipital locations. The pattern was not observed in adult head models.13 Studies adopting the scalp projection method indicate that the scalp-to-cortex distance is smaller in infants18,19 and children20 than in adults.13 Existing infant studies sampled a single age or a narrow age range. Hence, there is insufficient information about the differences across age on overall scalp-to-cortex distance or scalp-to-cortex distance by electrode positions.

1.2.

Scalp-Location-to-ROI Mapping Using Scalp Projection

The scalp projection method can also be used to identify the correspondence between scalp electrode locations and the underlying anatomical regions of interest (ROIs). The electrode-location-to-ROI mapping can be established first by transforming the individual’s own MRI and electrode locations to a canonical average MRI template which has an anatomical stereotaxic atlas. Scalp electrode locations can then be projected to the cortical surface to identify the corresponding ROI(s). Adult studies demonstrated that there was a relatively consistent correspondence between the 10–2013 and 10–1014 electrode positions and the underlying macroanatomical ROIs. A methodological challenge in the ROI mapping is the limited availability of subjects’ own MRIs. Coregistration methods for standalone DOT data have been developed so that electrode positions on the subject’s scalp can be transformed to the space of reference MRIs in a database.21,22 The coregistration methods result in similar results as using subjects’ own MRIs.4,22

Coregistering scalp electrode locations with reference MRIs is an effective method for identifying electrode-location-to-ROI correspondence in infants. One method is to project individuals’ electrode locations to a template brain with ROI parcellations constructed from a representative infant or an average template based on infant MRIs. Kabdebon et al.18 transformed infants’ (3 to 17 weeks) individual head models to a 7-week-old infant template and found stable correspondence between 10–20 electrode locations and underlying microanatomical ROIs. Electrodes O1, O2, T5, and T6 were projected to more inferior cortical regions compared with the projection regions (left and right middle occipital and middle temporal regions) reported by Okamoto et al.13 This study showed that age-related changes in the head and brain structures may lead to age-related variations in the correspondence between electrode positions and brain structures. Tsuzuki et al.23 transformed macroanatomical landmarks identified in 14 3- to 22-month-olds’ MRIs to a 12-month-old template18,23,24 in reference to virtual 10–10 electrode locations.11 They found intersubject variations in the relative positions among the microanatomical landmarks. However, the differences were smaller than the region defined by the 10–10 electrodes. Hence, there may be relatively stable electrode-location-to-ROI correspondence across ages. One important limitation of these studies is the use of a single individual or single-age template as a reference for age ranges across which significant brain development occurs (e.g., 3 to 17 weeks; or 3 to 22 months).

The use of individual MRIs or age-appropriate MRI templates and stereotaxic atlases for standalone DOT data can reduce errors in coregistration and provide more accurate representations of electrode-location-to-ROI correspondence. Lloyd-Fox et al.25 coregistered channel locations (midway of between adjacent optode/electrode locations) on both the infants’ (4.5- and 6-month-olds) own MRIs and the age-appropriate average templates from the Neurodevelopmental MRI Database.2629 Anatomical stereotaxic atlases were constructed for individuals’ own MRIs and age-appropriate average templates.30 The correspondence between channel locations and ROIs in individual MRIs was highly comparable with the correspondence identified in the age-appropriate average MRI templates. An alternative to coregistering with the subjects’ own MRIs or average templates is to use an MRI from another infant with a similar head size and age. A series of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) studies adopted Lloyd-Fox et al.’s25 coregistration method and positioned optode locations on close-head-size individual MRIs31 or both close MRIs and age-appropriate average templates.19,32,33 Scalp projection was used to establish probabilistic mappings between channel locations and atlas ROIs. The channels with a large probability of localizing to a target ROI were used for group-level analyses.

1.3.

Use of DOT Sensitivity to Describe Scalp-to-Cortex Correspondence

The use of scalp projection for determining electrode-location-to-ROI correspondence is limited. It does not consider the interaction between near-infrared light and the optical properties of the biological tissues. Therefore, the approach is limited to spatially contiguous anatomical areas and cannot directly model the extent of the cortical regions being measured by NIRS recording nor the intensity of the signal generated by blood flow. It is possible that spatial projection from channel location maps to a different ROI than the channel of photons traveling from the source to the detector through the head and brain. An alternative to the scalp projection is to use DOT sensitivity patterns to determine the correspondence between scalp recording locations and underlying cortical anatomy. DOT sensitivity represents the extent to which the DOT instrument can detect changes in brain activities in the region that it is sampling.34 DOT sensitivity can be quantified by the probability of a photon traveling from a source-optode location to a voxel inside the head (2-point Green’s function).35 We will call this the “direct DOT.” A second approach to quantify DOT sensitivity in NIRS source–detector recording by calculating the product of the fluence distribution at the source location and the detector location [photon measurement density function (PMDF);10 3-point Green’s function].35,36 We will call this the “source–detector channel DOT” and “S-D channel DOT.”37

The DOT sensitivity provides a measure of the scalp-location-to-ROI correspondence. NIRS channel location is defined as a point equidistant from the source and detector optodes. The direct DOT and S-D channel DOT fluence distributions can be used to estimate the distance from scalp locations to the surface of cortical regions that are measurable with DOT and localize the ROIs that would be sampled with NIRS.38 Studies estimated DOT sensitivity in head models with atlas parcellations in infants,3941 children,20 and adults.42 These studies computed the channel-to-ROI correspondence and generated look-up tables to show channel-to-ROI probabilities. Zimeo Morais et al.42 computed the specificity of each channel to the corresponding ROIs using the S-D channel DOT measure. Such look-up tables facilitate the optimization of channel array design to maximize DOT sensitivity to user-specified ROIs42,43 and help to localize the possible ROIs that generated the fNIRS signals.3941

1.4.

Present Study

This study examined age-related changes in scalp-to-brain distance and the correspondence between scalp locations and anatomical regions. Our primary contribution was to use existing methods for DOT sensitivity with age-appropriate, realistic head models that cover the infancy period (2 weeks to 2 years) with narrow age bands and compare these to older children (4 and 12 years) and adults (20 to 24 years). Our choices of age groups reflect the primary aim of this study of examining age-related differences throughout infancy. The child and adult age groups served as “reference” groups for delineating how scalp-to-cortex correspondence in infancy may differ from the patterns in childhood and adulthood.

We employed different methods for assessing scalp-to-cortex correspondence. The scalp projection was used to estimate the distances between scalp electrode and channel locations and the cortical surface. We projected the electrode location to atlas locations delineated on individual MRIs to identify the anatomical mapping between scalp and ROI locations. We also computed the distance between scalp locations and the cortical locations with maximum direct DOT and S-D channel DOT fluence. The DOT sensitivity methods extend from the scalp project method by modeling how light from the source optode travels through the head and brain. We use the name “electrode” and “channel” interchangeably, though the “electrode location” properly refers to the scalp projection method whereas the “channel location” properly refers to the S-D channel DOT method. We provided look-up tables to present the probabilistic mapping between scalp locations and the underlying anatomical ROIs. Accurate and age-specific descriptions of scalp-to-cortex distance and scalp-to-ROI mapping are the bases for designing developmentally sensitive channel placements for NIRS recordings. Our look-up tables also provide important references for researchers to make anatomical interpretations of NIRS results from their specific age groups.

2.

Method

2.1.

Participants

The participants were 1058 typically developing participants ranging from 2 weeks to 24 years of age. The same sample was studied in Ref. 37. The MRIs were obtained from open-access databases and a local scanning facility. The sample consisted of nine participants (four females) from the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE),44 280 (143 females) from the Baby Connectome Project (BCP),45 177 (93 females) from the Early Brain Development Study (EBDS),46 282 (106 females) from the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS),47 14 (5 females) from the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Data Repository (PING),48 and 296 scans (141 females) from data collected at the McCausland Center of Brain Imaging (MCBI) or drawn from collaborative studies at other sites. Table 1 presents the number of MRIs for the open-access databases, separately for age and gender. The sample ages were narrowest in the infancy period (2 weeks, 1-, 1.5-, 3-, or 6-month intervals from 2 weeks to 2 years) and included exemplar ages in children and adolescent ages (4 and 12 years) and adult (20 to 24 years). All studies had institutional review board approval and informed consent. The University of South Carolina Institutional Review Board approved data collection at the MacCausland Center for Brain Imaging (MCBI) and the use of data from all open-access databases.

Table 1

Demographical information of study participants by age group, sex, and data source.

Participant informationData source
Age groupTotal NFemale NABIDE NBCP NEBDS NIBIS NMCBI and collaboration sites NPING N
2 weeks41240338000
1 month964001779000
2 months68400860000
3 months382102400140
4.5 months542904100130
6 months743500060140
7.5 months611700049120
9 months60350480390
10.5 months42210400020
12 months1093600089200
15 months78410630870
18 months763103608320
2 years66220006510
4 years24900001014
12 years37149000280
20 to 24 years1347700001340
Note: ABIDE, Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange; BCP, Baby Connectome Project; EBDS, Early Brain Development Study; IBIS, Infant Brain Imaging Study; MCBI, McCausland Center for Brain Imaging; PING, Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Data Repository.

2.2.

MRI Sequences

This study utilized T1-weighted (T1W) and T2-weighted (T2W) scans from each collection site. An age-appropriate average template was constructed for each age group. Details of the MRI acquisition protocols have been described in the literature on the Neurodevelopmental MRI Database.2630,49 Details of the average template construction are also available in the literature and the Supplemental Material. All MRIs were converted to NIFTI compressed format with 32-bit floating point resolution. Data quality control and harmonization procedures were taken to ensure standardization among scans from different sites detailed in Refs. 27, 28, 29, and 49. MRI intensity variations found in different datasets were corrected. Bias-field inhomogeneity correction (N4 algorithm) was performed on the extracted T1W images.50,51 In addition, the peak of the gray matter (GM) intensity was normed to 100 to allow the scans from different sources to be standardized to the same voxel value range and resolution. All scans were visually inspected to ensure that there were no abnormalities in movement, image intensity, image orientation, or other artifacts.

2.3.

MRI Preprocessing and Segmentation

First, the brains were extracted from the whole-head MRI volume in a procedure adapted from the FSL VBM pipeline.52 The T1W volume for each participant was registered to an age-appropriate average MRI template. The MRI templates came from the Neurodevelopmental MRI Database.2729 The brain from the average template was transformed into the participant MRI space and used a mask on the head volume. The extracted masked data were then used with the FSL brain extraction tool program.53,54 Each brain was visually inspected and manually modified if necessary.

Second, each head MRI volume was segmented into 9 or 10 media types: GM, white matter (WM), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), nonmyelinated WM, other brain matter, skin, skull, air, eyes, and other inside skull materials. Optical properties would need to be set for each of the tissue types for Monte Carlo simulations described below. The FSL FAST procedure55 was used to segment the T1W images into GM, WM, or other matter (OM). The tentative GM/WM classification from the initial step is problematic for infants at 12 months of age or younger whose brain lacks myelination (both GM and nonmyelinated tissue types appear dark in T1W).8,28 For infants in these age groups, the pattern of GM/WM in the 2-year-old average MRI template was used as a probability map to distinguish nonmyelinated tissues that should later be myelinated axons (WM), those that were GM or other nonmyelinated tissue. The CSF was identified in the T2W images using a threshold procedure. The CSF was removed from the materials from the FAST procedure, with the remainder defined as GM, WM, nonmyelinated WM, or other inside skull materials. The BETSURF procedure53,54 was used with the extracted brain, T1W and T2W volumes, to identify skull and scalp regions. The nasal cavity and eyes were identified manually using MRIcron.56,57 Finally, any OM inside the head volume not defined as above was defined as “other inside skull material.” This generally was in the region of the neck and consisted primarily of muscle and secondarily of spinal bone. Our segmentation procedure for MRIs from participants ranging from 2 weeks to 89 years collected from different sites was validated in previous studies.28,29,49 The segmented regions were assembled into a single MRI volume we will refer to as the “segmented head MRI volume.” Figure 1(a) shows a 3D rendering of the T1W volume from a 3-month-old infant with a cutout revealing the segmented MRI volume. The realistic head model represents the geometry of the head and allows us to differentiate optical properties of different tissue types.

Fig. 1

Segmented head MRI volumes. The examples were taken from the same 3-month-old infant MRI. (a) The segmented head model. Aqua is the scalp, green is the nonbone structure (muscle, sinew, fat), gold is the nasal and mouth air cavities, turquoise is the skull, light blue is the dura, orange is the CSF, dark blue is the GM, and purple is the WM. (b) The segmented head model with dense finite element mesh. (c) The segmented head model with sparse finite element mesh.

NPH_8_3_035003_f001.png

2.4.

Mesh Generation

A finite element tetrahedral mesh was constructed from the segmented head MRI volume. Figures 1(b) and 1(c) displayed meshes that were produced using the iso2mesh toolbox with CGAL 3.6 mesh program (v2m function).58 Tetrahedral meshes accurately represent the boundaries of complex three-dimensional volumetric tissues and increase the accuracy in modeling photon propagation in complex mediums such as the head and brain.59 The finite element volumetric meshes have nodes that represent the voxel locations for the tetrahedra, a four-element matrix representing the corners of each tetrahedron, and a vector representing the media type from the segmented head MRI volume. A mesh was generated for each segmented head MRI volume. Figure 1(b) shows an example of the dense meshes. The number of nodes, elements, and tetra volume was calculated for the infants (2 weeks to 2 years), children (4 and 12 years), and adults (Fig. S1 in the Supplemental Material). We will refer to this as the “segmented finite element mesh.” We used the mesh to locate points on the scalp that were closest to the electrode positions and for the segmented finite element mesh for the MMC computer program.

2.5.

Scalp Locations

2.5.1.

Virtual electrodes placement

The locations for the 10–10 and 10–5 electrode systems were constructed on each head MRI volume. Figure 2 shows the placements of 10–10 and 10–5 electrode positions. First, we manually marked cranial fiducial points using MRIcron:56,57 nasion (Nz), Vz, inion (Iz), left preauricular point (LPA), right preauricular point (RPA), left mastoid, and right mastoid. The fiducial definitions were defined by procedures described in Refs. 11 and 60. The manual marking was done by one person (e.g., research assistant or one of the authors), and the marks were visually inspected by one of the authors for accuracy and corrected if necessary. The coordinates of the fiducials were transferred onto the scalp mesh. Next, we calculated 81 virtual electrode positions based on the unambiguously illustrated 10–10 system.11 Details for constructing the 10–10 locations are described in Ref. 60 and the Supplemental Material. The electrode positions were visually inspected by one of the authors. We simulated a total of 358 electrodes on 10–5 locations by calculating center points between 10–10 positions. The electrode positions also were computed for the average MRI templates.

Fig. 2

Virtual electrode placement. (a) 10–10 virtual electrode placement. From left to right: a 3-month individual head model, an average template for 3-month-olds, and a 2D layout of the 10–10 system. 10–10 electrodes were divided into four groups based on circumference location and expected similarity of scalp-to-cortex distances. The electrodes were further divided based on the hemisphere in the analysis that examined hemispheric differences in scalp-to-cortex distances. The groups were color-coded (fill colors) in circumference-location-by-hemisphere divisions. Electrodes were divided into frontal electrodes (F electrodes; red outline) and central/posterior (C, T, P, and O electrodes; blue outline) to examine the anterior-to-posterior differences in scalp-to-cortex distances. See text for an explanation of the electrode groupings. (b) 10–5 virtual electrode placement. From left to right: the same 3-month individual head model and the 3-month average template.

NPH_8_3_035003_f002.png

The individual electrode locations were divided into groups for analyses based on anatomical landmarks.11 There were divided into four groups based on sagittal curves, with groups of electrodes that we expected to have similar scalp-to-cortex distances. Group 1: electrodes on the central curve (Nz-Cz-Iz). They are over the interhemispheric fissure. Group 2: electrodes on the left and right curves between Nz and Iz (left: N1-LPA/T9-I1; right: N2-RPA/T10-I2). Group 3: electrodes on the left and right curves between Fpz and Oz (left: Fp1-T7-O1; right: Fp2-T8-O2). Group 4: the remaining electrodes enclosed by the central curve and the left and right curves between Fpz and Oz. We expected the first two electrode groups to have greater distances based on their lowest position on the scalp; group 3 to have intermediate distances; group 4 to have the smallest distances. The electrode groups were further divided based on the hemisphere in the analysis that explored hemispheric differences in distances. Electrode locations in groups 3 and 4 could also be divided by the coronal curve (T7-T8). We group these electrodes into frontal electrodes (F electrodes) and central/posterior electrodes (C, T, P, and O electrodes) for the analysis that examined the frontal-to-occipital differences in distances. The electrode groups are displayed in Fig. 2.

2.5.2.

Source–detector channels

Source–detector channel locations were defined using the electrode combinations centered on each 10–10 electrode. The 10–10 electrode locations were centered between surrounding adjacent pairs of 10–10 or 10–5 electrode locations. There were 251 source–detector pairs formed with adjacent 10–10 electrodes, MeanSeparation=58.0  mm; SD=14.2, and 251 pairs formed with adjacent 10–5 electrodes, MeanSeparation=28.9  mm; SD=7.1. The channel locations were used to estimate the “S-D channel DOT fluence” described below.

2.6.

Cortical Locations

Three stereotaxic atlases were constructed for each individual MRI. The atlases delineate cortical lobes or more specified locations within the lobes. We created a 1.5-cm spherical mask around each electrode or channel location to standardize the region to be identified in the atlases. The atlases and spherical masks were used with the scalp projection method (described below) to describe the correspondence between the scalp electrode positions and corresponding cortical structures in the individual’s own brain space with various spatial resolutions.25 They were also used with the “S-D channel DOT” estimates (described below) to identify the anatomical structures being sampled by the fluence distribution.

2.6.1.

Stereotaxic atlases

Three atlases were constructed for each individual MRI to delineate anatomical regions that can be used to identify brain locations corresponding to scalp electrode positions or DOT sensitivity patterns. Details of the atlas constructions may be found elsewhere.25,30,61 The first atlas was the LONI Probabilistic Brain Atlas (LPBA40),62 which contains 56 areas from the cortical and subcortical regions, brainstem, and cerebellum. The second was the Hammers atlas, based on MRIs from the IXI MRI database,63 which consists of 83 areas defined from the cortex, subcortical, brainstem, and cerebellum.64 We used a majority vote fusion procedure that combines labeled segments from manually segmented MRIs to produce atlases that identify an anatomical area for each brain voxel of the individual MRI. The third atlas was an automatically constructed lobar atlas that identifies the major cortical lobes (e.g., frontal), some sublobar cortical (e.g., fusiform gyrus), subcortical (e.g., striatum), cerebellum, and brainstem. The atlas was constructed by extracting and combining common areas from the LPBA40 and Hammers atlases constructed in the average template space. The individual MRI was linearly registered to the age-appropriate average template, and the age-appropriate lobar atlas was transformed using the linear registration matrix into the individual MRI space. Linear registration was performed using the FSL FLIRT function.65 The approach has produced good correspondence between the manually segmented average template atlas and the transformed atlas.25,30,61

2.6.2.

Spherical masks

We created a sphere with a 1.5-cm radius around each 10–10 electrode and channel location. The decision of the sphere size was based on previous studies.32,33 It is hypothesized that the DOT fluence distribution reaches about 3 cm deep into the brain with a 2-cm source–detector channel separation. Figure 3 shows examples of the spherical volumes that were used as masks for determining the correspondence between the scalp and brain locations. We identified the atlas ROIs that intersected with the masks and computed the voxel numbers in the sphere of these ROIs. Hence, there was a distribution of atlas ROIs with various voxel numbers that were mapped to each electrode and channel location.

Fig. 3

Methods for determining scalp-location-to-ROI mapping. The MRI was taken from a 3-month infant. (a) An example of a 1.5-cm-radius spherical mask created around an electrode location for the spatial scalp projection procedure. (b) An illustration of a spherical mask created around a source location for the direct DOT procedure. (c) An illustration of a spherical mask created around a channel location for the S-D channel DOT procedure. Monte Carlo photon migration simulations were used to estimate fluence distributions for individual 10–5 electrode locations (direct DOT) and 10–10 channel locations (S-D channel DOT). The red area represents greater fluence.

NPH_8_3_035003_f003.png

2.7.

DOT Sensitivity Analyses

The DOT fluence distribution was estimated from Monte Carlo simulations using the Monte Carlo eXtreme package MCX.66,67 Details of the simulations are presented in Ref. 37 and the Supplemental Material. The output from the Monte Carlo simulation contained the fluence across the entire MRI volume separately for each optode. Figure 3(b) shows the direct DOT fluence plotted on an individual subject MRI for a single source-optode location. We computed S-D channel DOT fluence by multiplying the source-optode fluence distribution by the detector-optode fluence distribution. This represents the DOT fluence sensitivity for a photon channel from the source to the detector (PMDF;10 or 3-point Green’s function).35,36 Figure 3(c) shows the S-D channel DOT fluence plotted on an MRI for a single source–detector pair.

2.8.

Spatial Scalp Projection

We performed scalp projections from each 10–10 or 10–5 electrode position on the scalp to the brain surface. These projections were used to measure the distance between each scalp electrode and the brain surface and to examine electrode scalp location to anatomical ROI correlation.19,25,3133 The brain locations underlying each electrode were determined by an algorithm in which a spatial projection from the electrode scalp surface to the brain center was done. The point where the projection intersected the cortex was identified. These projections were done with the scalp and brain elements of the segmented finite element mesh and mesh manipulation tools from the iso2mesh toolbox.58

2.9.

Scalp-to-Cortex Distance

We computed the distance from scalp electrode or channel locations to cortical surface for each individual from all groups. Figure 3(a) shows a 1.5-cm radius spherical mask that was created at the point where the scalp projection intersected the brain. The scalp projection distance was the distance between the scalp electrode position and the closest intersection point on the brain surface. The direct DOT distance was based on the direct DOT fluence. Figure 3(b) shows the spherical mask overlaid on the direct DOT fluence distribution at a source–electrode (optode) location. The maximum direct DOT fluence in the spherical mask was identified. The direct DOT distance was the distance between the scalp electrode position and the location of the brain voxel with the maximum direct DOT fluence in the spherical mask. The S-D channel DOT distance was based on the S-D channel DOT fluence. Figure 3(c) shows the spherical mask overlaid on the S-D channel DOT fluence distribution at a source–detector channel location. The S-D channel DOT distance was the distance between the scalp channel position and the location of the voxel with the maximum S-D channel DOT fluence in the spherical mask.

Data analyses examined the effect of age, estimation method, and electrode location on the scalp-to-cortex distance. A three-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the main effect of the three factors and their interaction effects. This was followed by additional analyses assessing the age-related differences in the distance measure across estimation methods, and the electrode-location differences in distances across estimation methods in infants, children, and adults. Post-hoc analyses were also performed to explore hemispheric asymmetry in scalp projection and S-D channel DOT distances across electrode positions and age groups.

2.10.

Scalp-Location-to-ROI Mapping

The scalp projection, direct DOT, and the S-D channel DOT sensitivity were used to generate a look-up procedure that links the scalp electrode or channel locations to the lobar, Hammer, and LPBA40 atlas ROIs. A 1.5-cm mask was placed on the scalp projection to the brain intersection, maximum point of the direct DOT fluence, or maximum point of the S-D channel DOT fluence. Figure 3(a) shows a sphere surrounding the scalp projection to the brain intersection; Fig. 3(b) shows a sphere surrounding the source-optode to the maximum direct DOT fluence location; Fig. 3(c) shows a sphere surrounding the channel location to the S-D channel DOT fluence. The anatomical ROI(s) of each voxel in the respective spheres were recorded. The percentage of voxels in each ROI that intersected with the spherical masks was computed. We created tables for each age that listed the ROIs and the percentage of voxels in each ROI that intersected with the spherical masks created around the 10–10 electrode (scalp projection) and channel locations (S-D channel DOT). The scalp projection look-up table details the spatial correspondence between the scalp electrode location and anatomical brain regions. The S-D channel DOT look-up table illustrates the S-D channel DOT sensitivity to cortical regions. These tables serve as references for NIRS or fNIRS users to make age-specific decisions of optode placement or evaluate channel-to-ROI mapping for existing channel configurations.

2.11.

Additional Measures and Analyses

Several methods and results are presented in the Supplemental Material. These include tMCimg5 simulations in all individual MRIs and age-appropriate average templates and MMC simulations59,66,68 in 3-month- and 6-month-old individual MRIs and average templates. We compared the scalp-to-cortex distance estimations obtained from the MCX, tMCimg, and MMC simulation packages. fNIRS optodes’ location decider (fOLD) channels: We used a set of electrode pairs to define source–detector channels from the 130 channel locations specified in the fOLD.42 We computed S-D channel DOT fluence in selected age groups (3 months, 6 months, and 20 to 24 years) for the 130 channel locations using fluence estimated from MCX simulations.42 We provided a look-up table that presented the specificity of each fOLD channel to the underlying ROI(s). We also compared scalp-to-cortex distances among simulation methods and brain model types.

3.

Results

3.1.

Scalp-to-Cortex Distance

The scalp-to-cortex distances were analyzed as a function of age group, estimation method, and electrode group. Parameter estimates from all ANOVA models were displayed in Table S2 in the Supplemental Material. Figure 4 shows the average scalp-to-cortex distances by age groups, estimation methods, and electrode groups. A three-way ANOVA (model 1) was performed to examine the effect of age group (2 weeks to 20 to 24 years), electrode group (four groups), estimation method (scalp projection, direct DOT, S-D channel DOT), and their interaction effects. There was a significant main effect of age group, F(15,12,144)=1652.55, p<0.001, electrode group, F(6,12,144)=14,821.90, p<0.001, and estimation method, F(2,12,144)=1150.78, p<0.001. There was a significant interaction effect of age-by-electrode-group, F(45,12,144)=18.34, p<0.001, age-by-method, F(30,12,144)=4.65, p<0.001, method-by-electrode-group, F(6,12,144)=107.26, p<0.001, and age-by-electrode-group-by-method three-way interaction, F(90,12,144)=1.59, p<0.001. The mean distance averaged across estimation methods and electrode locations was greatest in the adults, followed by 12-year-olds, p values <0.001. The mean distance at age 4 years was greater than the distance at most of the infant and toddler ages, p values <0.05, except for 3-month- and 4.5-month-olds. The youngest age group (2 weeks) had the smallest distance, p values <0.001. However, the mean distance did not show a systematic increase with age during infancy. The mean distance across age groups and estimation methods was furthest from the cortex at the left- and right-lowest circumference electrode positions (group 2), followed by the midline electrodes (group 1), the second-lowest circumference electrodes (group 3), and the remaining electrodes (group 4), p values <0.001. The mean distance averaged across age groups and electrode locations was largest for the S-D channel DOT estimation method, followed by scalp projection, and direct DOT in descending order, p values <0.001. Figure 4 shows that the age differences among infant and toddler groups (2 weeks to 2 years) differed by estimation methods and electrode locations. Figure S3 in the Supplemental Material provides an additional visualization of distances across individual electrode positions estimated using the three methods. We conducted two sets of follow-up analyses (models 2a, 2b, 3a, 3b) to further examine the age-by-electrode-location interaction effect on the distances estimated using scalp projection and S-D channel DOT, as the two methods have direct applications to NIRS measurements.

Fig. 4

Mean scalp-to-cortex distances averaged across individual electrode locations by age group, estimation method, and electrode group. The distances were estimated using individual MRIs.

NPH_8_3_035003_f004.png

We further examined how scalp-to-cortex distances varied across scalp positions and age groups. Figure 5 shows two-dimensional (2D) scalp topographical maps plotted with the scalp projection distance represented by the color map separately by age, and 3D renderings of the distance on the head for three selected ages. Figure 5 confirmed the main effect of age group and electrode group found in the previous analysis. The figure further revealed variations in distance between anterior/frontal positions and posterior positions. The shift from the frontal-midline electrodes to the other scalp positions was evident in several groups. Figure 6 shows 2D maps and 3D renderings for the S-D channel DOT scalp-to-cortex distance. The distance of the maximum S-D channel DOT fluence was larger than the scalp projections distance at all ages as shown in the previous analyses (c.f. Fig. 4). The anterior-to-posterior variation was also found with the S-D channel DOT distance for most of the age groups.

Fig. 5

Mean distance from the scalp to the cortical surface by electrode locations estimated using scalp projection. For the visualization purpose, estimations for the 10–5 electrode locations were displayed. The color bar denotes the distance range for all figure types. (a) Scalp topographical maps for all age groups. Darker red represents closer scalp-to-cortex distances, and darker blue indicates greater distances. (b) 3D rendering of mean distances on the heads (top row) and on the brains (bottom row) at 3 months, 9 months, and 2 years, selected as examples. Head and brain models were generated using age-matched average templates.

NPH_8_3_035003_f005.png

Fig. 6

Mean S-D channel DOT distances at 10–10 channel positions. The color bar denotes the distance range for all figure types. (a) Scalp topographical maps for all age groups. Darker red represents closer scalp-to-cortex distances, and darker blue indicates greater distances. (b) 3D rendering of mean distance on the heads (top row) and the brains (bottom row) at 3 months, 9 months, and 12 years, selected as examples. Head and brain models were generated using age-matched average templates.

NPH_8_3_035003_f006.png

We conducted two-way ANOVAs to depict the age-by-electrode-location interaction effects. As described in Sec. 2.5.1, we regrouped the electrodes in groups 3 and 4 with the purpose of exploring the anterior-to-posterior variation in distance. The first ANOVA (model 2a) tested the effect of individual age groups and electrode groups on scalp projection distances. There was a significant effect of age groups, F(15,3816)=374.64, p<0.001, electrode group F(3,3816)=6393.45, p<0.001, and age-by-electrode-group interaction effect, F(45,3816)=7.54, p<0.001. The scalp-to-cortex distances of the group two (lowest-circumference) electrodes were the largest, followed by group one (midline) electrodes for all age groups, p values <0.001. The group three electrodes at frontal locations were more distant to the cortex than group four electrodes at central, temporal, parietal, and occipital locations for all infant age groups, p values <0.001. The difference between groups 2 and 3 electrode locations was not significant for 4 years, 12 years, and 20 to 24 years. The second ANOVA (model 2b) tested the effect of age groups and electrode groups on S-D channel DOT distances. There was a significant effect of age groups, F(15,4164)=690.39, p<0.001, electrode group F(3,4164)=3517.76, p<0.001, and age-by-electrode-group interaction effect, F(45,4164)=12.69, p<0.001. The distances at group 2 (lowest-circumference) electrodes were the largest for all age groups, p values <0.001. Group 1 (midline) electrodes were more distant to the cortex than both group 3 (frontal) and group 4 (central, temporal, parietal, and occipital) electrodes for most of the age groups, p values <0.05, but the distances between group 1 and group 3 electrodes were not significantly different for 3 months, 4.5 months, 6 months, and 7.5 months. Group 3 electrodes had a greater distance than group 4 electrodes, p values <0.05, except for 2 weeks, 1 month, 2 months, 18 months, 2 years, and 4 years. The findings indicate that the differences in scalp-to-cortex distances between anterior/midline and posterior electrode positions varied by age and estimation methods. There was an anterior/midline-to-posterior decrease in scalp-to-cortex in most of the infant groups.

We investigated whether scalp-to-cortex distances varied by circumference location and hemisphere across age groups. The analyses excluded electrodes in the midline positions (group 1). Figure 7 displays mean distances by age group, hemisphere, and electrode group for the scalp projection distance [Fig. 7(a)] and S-D channel DOT estimations [Fig. 7(b)]. The first three-way ANOVA (model 3a) tested the effect of age groups, electrode group, and hemisphere (left and right) on the scalp projection distances. There was a significant effect of age group, F(15,5724)=432.03, p<0.001, electrode group, F(2,5724)=15,661.50, p<0.001, and age-by-electrode-group interaction, F(30,5724)=11.39, p<0.001. These findings are consistent with the previous results. There was a significant effect of hemisphere, F(1,5724)=19.35, p<0.001. The mean distance in the right hemisphere was greater than the left hemisphere, p<0.001. The age-by-hemisphere interaction effect, electrode-group-by-hemisphere effect, nor the three-way interaction effect was significant. The second three-way ANOVA (model 3b) examined the effect of age group, electrode group, and hemisphere on the S-D channel DOT distances. There was a significant effect of age group, F(15,6246)=870.31, p<0.001, electrode group, F(2,6246)=9248.99, p<0.001, and age-by-electrode-group interaction, F(30,6246)=14.87, p<0.001. In addition, there was a significant effect of hemisphere, F(1,6246)=44.49, p<0.001, age-by-hemisphere interaction, F(15,6246)=6.20, p<0.001, and electrode-group-by-hemisphere interaction effect, F(2,6246)=3.14, p<0.04. The age-by-electrode-group-hemisphere interaction effect was not significant. Across all electrode groups, the mean distance was greater in the left than right hemisphere for 6 months, 7.5 months, 12 months, 4 years, 12 years, and 20 to 24 years, p values <0.05, whereas the hemispheric difference was not significant for the rest of the age groups. Across all age groups, the distance was greater in the left than right hemisphere for group 3 (second-lowest circumference) and group 4 (remaining) electrodes, p values <0.001, but not for group 2 (lowest circumference) electrodes.

Fig. 7

Mean scalp-to-cortex distances across individual 10–10 electrode/channel locations by circumference locations, and hemispheres for individual age groups. Electrode locations at group one (c.f. Fig. 2) were excluded from the analyses. (a) Mean scalp projection distance across individual electrode locations by circumference locations and hemispheres for individual age groups. (b) Mean S-D channel DOT distance across individual channel locations by circumference locations and hemispheres for individual age groups.

NPH_8_3_035003_f007.png

3.2.

Additional Analyses on Scalp-to-Cortex Distance

The Supplemental Material provides additional results on the effect of sex, head model type, and additional estimation methods on age-related differences in scalp-to-cortex distance. Figure S4 in the Supplemental Material displays the effects of sex, age group, and electrode group on S-D channel DOT distances estimated using individual head models. It shows that the S-D channel DOT distances were greater in males than females in some age groups but not others. Figure S5 in the Supplemental Material shows that the S-D channel DOT distances by age group and electrode group computed from age-matched average templates (Fig. S5A in the Supplemental Material) and individual head models (Fig. S5B in the Supplemental Material). The distances for the participant-based averages (cf. Fig. 4) were similar to the distances calculated from the average MRI template for the same age. Figures S6 and S7 in the Supplemental Material show comparisons among scalp-to-cortex distances estimated using MCX, tMCimg, and MMC simulation methods with 10–10 channel locations, as well as MCX simulation with fOLD channel locations. They showed that MCX, MMC, and tMCimg estimation methods produced comparable S-D channel DOT distances for individual MRIs and average templates at 3 months and 6 months. The distances estimated using MCX fOLD with average templates were larger than distances calculated from other methods with individual head models and average templates at the example age groups.

3.3.

Scalp-Location-to-ROI Mapping

The scalp projection and the S-D channel DOT sensitivity were used to generate look-up tables that show the cortical ROIs for each of the 10–10 electrodes. A supplemental Excel spreadsheet is presented with this information (see Supplemental Table S3). This table contains each estimation type and age combination (e.g., spatial projection 2-0 weeks, spatial projection 1-0 month, source–detector DOT 2-0 weeks, source–detector DOT 1-month, etc.). Each table has one row for each electrode containing columns for the electrode name and the cortical ROIs for the lobar, Hammers, and LPBA40 atlases. These tables could be used to find a specific scalp-location-to-ROI combination for each of the ages in the study. The scalp projection and S-D channel DOT estimations yielded overlapping and considerably different scalp-location-to-ROI mappings. For example, between-method discrepancy was found at 32 scalp locations where the electrode was mapped to at least one different ROI for the 3-month group. The discrepancy was found at 29 scalp locations for the 20- to 24-year-olds. NIRS/fNIRS users are recommended to refer to the S-D channel DOT look-up table, as the channel-to-ROI correspondence was estimated based on the sensitivity of the channel location to the brain region.

The channel-location-to-ROI correspondences showed stability over age for some of the channel locations based on the S-D channel DOT estimation. Table 2 and Fig. 8 summarize the differences in channel-to-ROI mapping across age groups. For each channel location, the table listed ROIs that mapped to the location with sufficient confidence (defined as having at least 25% of brain voxels intersected with the 1.5-cm-radius sphere created around the channel location) and the age groups with the ROI that survived the threshold. The channel locations were color-coded to show different levels of between-age consistency in Fig. 8. The lobar atlas has macrostructural segmentations which resulted in similar channel-to-ROI correspondence across ages. Most of the channel locations (52 out of 81 channels) were mapped to one lobar atlas ROI for all age groups. Some channels were sensitive to more than one lobar ROIs in younger age groups but were mapped to one lobar ROI in older age groups (CZ, PZ, OZ, F9, F10, T10, FT7, FT8, O1, O2, C5, C6, C1, C2, CP5, and CP6). For example, O1 (left) and O2 (right) were mapped to the cerebellum, fusiform gyrus, occipital lobe, and parietal lobe for the infant groups but only to the occipital lobe for 20- to 24-year-olds. Some channels were sensitive to more than one lobar ROIs only in older age groups (POZ, PO9, I1, and I2). For example, I2 corresponded to both the right cerebellum and occipital lobe in 12- and 20- to 24-year-olds but only to the cerebellum in younger ages. Other channels had more discrepant channel-to-ROI mappings over age (IZ, P7, P8, PO7, PO8, C3, C4, PO3, and PO4).

Table 2

Atlas locations corresponding to the scalp channel locations estimated using S-D channel DOT method. For each channel location, we listed atlas ROI that had at least 25% of its voxels intersected with the 1.5-cm-radius sphere created around the channel location (c.f. Supplemental Table S3). We also listed the age groups with the ROI that survived the threshold.

Electrode numberElectrode nameLobar atlas ROIAgesHammer atlas ROIAgesLPBA40 atlas ROIAges
1NZFrontalAllMedial orbital gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months, 20 to 24 years
2FPZFrontalAllSuperior frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 months
Superior frontal gyrusAll
3AFZFrontalAllSuperior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
4FZFrontalAllSuperior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
5FCZFrontalAllSuperior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
6CZFrontalAllPostcentral gyrus1mPrecentral gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months; 7.5 months to 20 to 24 years
Parietal2 weeks to 2 monthsPrecentral gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Superior frontal gyrus3 to 12 months; 18 months to 20 to 24 years
Superior parietal gyrus2 weeks
7CPZParietalAllPostcentral gyrus6 months; 12 years, 20 to 24 yearsPostcentral gyrus3 months to 20 to 24 years
Superior parietal gyrusAllPrecuneus2 weeks to 3 months; 15 months
Superior parietal gyrusAll
8PZOccipital2 weeks; 1 monthCuneus2 weeks; 1 monthPrecuneus2 months to 20 to 24 years
ParietalAllSuperior parietal gyrusAllSuperior parietal gyrus1 months to 20 to 24 years
9POZOccipitalAllCuneusAllCuneusAll
Parietal20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 2 months; 7.5, 12 monthsSuperior occipital gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Superior parietal gyrus12 years
10OZFusiform2 weeks, 1, 3, 4.5, 9, 10.5, 15, and 18 monthsCuneus2 months to 20 to 24 yearsCuneus3 months to 20 to 24 years
OccipitalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllLingual gyrus1, 3, and 4.5 months
Lingual gyrus2 weeks, 1, 3, and 4.5 monthsMiddle occipital gyrus2 months, 4.5 months to 4 years, 20 to 24 years
11IZCerebellum2 weeks to 4 yearsCerebellum2 weeks to 12 yearsCerebellum2 weeks to 12 years
Fusiform10.5 months, 2 y, 4, 20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe12, 20 to 24 yearsLingual gyrus12, 20 to 24 years
Occipital12, 20 to 24 yearsLingual gyrus12, 20 to 24 years
12N1FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months
Medial orbital gyrusAllMiddle orbitofrontal gyrus4.5 months to 20 to 24 years
Superior frontal gyrus2 weeks; 1 monthSuperior frontal gyrusAll
13AF9FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrus2 weeks to 9 months; 12 months, 2 years, 4 yearsLateral orbitofrontal gyrus4.5, 10.5, 15 months; 2 years to 20 to 24 years
Lateral orbital gyrus4.5, 10.5, 15 months; 2 years to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle orbitofrontal gyrus3 to 12 months; 18 months to 4 years
Medial orbital gyrus12 months
14F9Frontal2 weeks to 3 monthsAnterior temporal lobe, lateral part4.5 months to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle temporal gyrus10.5, 15 months; 20 to 24 years
TemporalAllAnterior temporal lobe, medial part12 and 18 months, 2 and 4 yearsSuperior temporal gyrusAll
Superior temporal gyrus, anterior part9, 10.5, 15 months, 12 years, 20 to 24 years
15FT9TemporalAllAnterior temporal lobe, lateral part3 months to 20 to 24 yearsInferior temporal gyrusAll
Medial and inferior temporal gyriAllMiddle temporal gyrus1 month to 20 to 24 years
Superior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 3 months
16T9TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyriAllInferior temporal gyrusAll
Middle temporal gyrus2 weeks to 3 months; 20 to 24 years
17TP9CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
18P9CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
19PO9CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
Parietal12 years; 20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe12 years; 20 to 24 yearsInferior occipital gyrus12 years; 20 to 24 years
20I1CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
Occipital12 years; 20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe6 months; 12 years; 20 to 24 yearsInferior occipital gyrus6 and 10.5 months; 12 years; 20 to 24 years
Parietal12 years
21N2FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrus3, 6, 7.5 months; 12 years; 20 to 24 yearsMiddle orbitofrontal gyrus6, 7.5 months; 2 years to 20 to 24 years
Medial orbital gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months
22AF10FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrusAllLateral orbitofrontal gyrus12 years; 20 to 24 years
Lateral orbital gyrus12 years; 20 to 24 yearsMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks, 1 month
Medial orbital gyrus7.5 to 12 months; 18 months; 2 yearsMiddle orbitofrontal gyrus1 month to 12 years
Superior frontal gyrus12 months
23F10Frontal2 weeks to 7.5 monthsAnterior temporal lobe, lateral part9, 10.5, 15 months; 2, 12, 20 to 24 yearsMiddle temporal gyrus20 to 24 years
TemporalAllAnterior temporal lobe, medial part4.5 months to 4 yearsSuperior temporal gyrusAll
Superior temporal gyrus, anterior part9, 15, 18 months; 4 years to 20 to 24 years
24FT10TemporalAllAnterior temporal lobe, lateral part3 months to 20 to 24 yearsInferior temporal gyrusAll
Anterior temporal lobe, medial part1, 2, 7.5, 12 monthsMiddle temporal gyrus3 months to 20 to 24 years
Medial and inferior temporal gyri6, 9, 10.5, 15 months; 2, 12, 20 to 24 yearsSuperior temporal gyrus1 to 3, 6, 7.5 months
25T10Cerebellum10.5 and 15 monthsCerebellum10.5 and 15 monthsCerebellum10.5 and 15 months
TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyriAllInferior temporal gyrusAll
Middle temporal gyrus2 weeks to 3 months; 6 months; 20 to 24 years
26TP10CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
27P10CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
28PO10CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
Lateral remainder of occipital lobe12, 20 to 24 yearsInferior occipital gyrus12, 20 to 24 years
29I2CerebellumAllCerebellumAllCerebellumAll
Occipital12, 20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe6, 7.5 months; 2 to 20 to 24 yearsInferior occipital gyrus6, 10.5 months; 2 to 20 to 24 years
Middle occipital gyrus20 to 24 years
30FP1FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrus2 yearsMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrus2 weeks to 2 years; 12 years, 20 to 24 years
31AF7FrontalAllLateral orbital gyrus7.5 months; 2, 4 yearsInferior frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks to 10.5 months; 18 months
32F7FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Lateral orbital gyrus6, 7.5 months; 2 yearsLateral orbitofrontal gyrus4.5, 6, 9 months to 12 years
33FT7Frontal2 weeks to 3 monthsMedial and inferior temporal gyri4 yearsMiddle temporal gyrusAll
TemporalAllSuperior temporal gyrus, anterior part3 months to 20 to 24 yearsSuperior temporal gyrusAll
34T7TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyriAllInferior temporal gyrus4.5 months
Superior temporal gyrus, central part2 monthsMiddle temporal gyrusAll
Superior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 3 months; 10.5 months
35TP7TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyri2 weeks, 3 monthsInferior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 12 years
Posterior temporal lobeAllMiddle temporal gyrusAll
36P7Cerebellum2 weeks, 1 monthCerebellum2 weeks, 1 monthCerebellum2 weeks, 1 month
Parietal6 months, 2, 12, 20 to 24 yearsLateral remainder of occipital lobe6 to 10.5 months; 15 months to 4 yearsInferior temporal gyrus4.5 to 7.5 months; 12, 18 months to 4 years
TemporalAllPosterior temporal lobeAllMiddle occipital gyrus10.5 to 15 months; 2, 20 to 24 years
Middle temporal gyrus6, 12 months; 12, 20 to 24 years
37PO7Cerebellum2 weeks to 2 months; 4.5 monthsCerebellum2 weeks, 1 monthCerebellum2 weeks, 1 month
Fusiform10.5, 15 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllInferior occipital gyrus2 weeks to 4 years
Occipital6, 12 months; 2, 4 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Parietal2 month to 20 to 24 years
38O1Cerebellum2 weeks; 1 monthCerebellum2 weeksCerebellum2 weeks
Fusiform4.5, 9, 15, 18 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllInferior occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1, 4.5 months
Occipital1 month to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle occipital gyrusAll
Parietal3, 4.5, 7.5, 18 months, 12 years
39FP2FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrus7.5 months; 2 yearsMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrus1, 2 months
Superior frontal gyrusAll
40AF8FrontalAllAnterior orbital gyrus6 to 12 months; 18 months, 2 yearsInferior frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks to 12 years
41F8FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Lateral orbital gyrus4.5 months to 4 yearsLateral orbitofrontal gyrus3 months to 12 years
42FT8Frontal2 weeks to 2 months; 6 monthsMedial and inferior temporal gyri20 to 24 yearsMiddle temporal gyrus3, 4.5, 9, 10.5, 15 months; 12, 20 to 24 years
TemporalAllSuperior temporal gyrus, anterior part3 months to 20 to 24 yearsSuperior temporal gyrusAll
43T8TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyriAllInferior temporal gyrus10.5 months
Middle temporal gyrusAll
Superior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 6, 7.5 months; 4 years
44TP8TemporalAllMedial and inferior temporal gyri2 to 7.5 monthsInferior temporal gyrusAll
Posterior temporal lobeAllMiddle temporal gyrusAll
45P8Cerebellum2 weeks to 2 monthsCerebellum2 weeks, 1 monthCerebellum2 weeks, 1 month
Fusiform15 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobe10.5, 15, 18 months; 20 to 24 yearsInferior temporal gyrus6 to 12 months; 18 months to 4 years
Parietal6 months; 4 to 20 to 24 yearsPosterior temporal lobeAllMiddle occipital gyrus20 to 24 years
TemporalAllMiddle temporal gyrus2 months; 6 to 12 months; 18 months to 20 to 24 years
46PO8Cerebellum2 weeks to 2 monthsCerebellum2 weeks, 1 monthCerebellum2 weeks, 1 month
Fusiform9, 10.5, 15, 18 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllInferior occipital gyrus2 weeks to 2 years
Occipital12 months; 2, 4, 12 to 24 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus1 month to 20 to 24 years
Parietal3 to 12, 18 months to 20 to 24 years
47O2Cerebellum2 weeks to 2 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllInferior occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1, 4.5 months
Fusiform9, 10.5, 15, 18 monthsMiddle occipital gyrusAll
OccipitalAll
Parietal3, 4.5, 7.5 months
48AF3FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrusAll
49AF4FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 6 months
50F5FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
51F3FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrus2 weeks; 4.5 to 12 months; 4 years
Middle frontal gyrusAll
52F1FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks to 12 yearsMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months; 7.5 months to 2 years; 12, 20 to 24 years
53F2FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks to 4 yearsMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
54F4FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
55F6FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Middle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
56FC5FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Precentral gyrusAllPrecentral gyrusAll
57FC3FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Precentral gyrus2 weeks to 4 yearsPrecentral gyrus2 weeks to 12 years
58FC1FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrus2 weeks; 2 months to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Precentral gyrus2 weeks; 1 monthPrecentral gyrus2 weeks, 1 month
Superior frontal gyrusAllSuperior frontal gyrusAll
59FC2FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrus1 month to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle frontal gyrus1 month to 20 to 24 years
Precentral gyrus2 weeksSuperior frontal gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrusAll
60FC4FrontalAllMiddle frontal gyrusAllMiddle frontal gyrusAll
Precentral gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 10.5, 12, 15 monthsPrecentral gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 9, 12, 15 months; 12 years
61FC6FrontalAllInferior frontal gyrusAllInferior frontal gyrusAll
Precentral gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 9 months to 20 to 24 yearsPrecentral gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 9 months to 20 to 24 years
62C5ParietalAllPostcentral gyrus3 months to 20 to 24 yearsPostcentral gyrus4.5 months to 20 to 24 years
Temporal2 weeks to 4 yearsRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)1, 10.5, 15 months; 2, 4, 20 to 24 yearsSuperior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 4 years
Superior temporal gyrus, central part3 to 9 months; 12 months to 4 yearsSupramarginal gyrus15 months, 20 to 24 years
63C3Frontal3, 6 to 10.6, 18 months; 12, 20 to 24 yearsPostcentral gyrusAllPostcentral gyrusAll
ParietalAllPrecentral gyrus3, 6, 7.5 months; 12 yearsPrecentral gyrus3 to 10.5 months; 12 years
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 weeks to 4 years, 20 to 24 yearsSupramarginal gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months; 7.5, 9, 12 months to 4 years
64C1FrontalAllPostcentral gyrus2 weeks to 4.5, 7.5 months to 4 yearsPostcentral gyrus2 weeks to 2, 7.5, 12 months to 4 years
Parietal2 weeks to 4 yearsPrecentral gyrusAllPrecentral gyrusAll
Superior parietal gyrus1 monthSuperior parietal gyrus1 month
65C2FrontalAllPostcentral gyrus2 weeks to 2 months; 12, 15 monthsPostcentral gyrus2 weeks; 1, 15 months
Parietal2 weeks to 3 months; 12, 15 monthsPrecentral gyrusAllPrecentral gyrusAll
Superior frontal gyrus4.5, 6, 9 months
Superior parietal gyrus2 weeks
66C4Frontal2 weeks; 2 months to 20 to 24 yearsPostcentral gyrusAllPostcentral gyrusAll
ParietalAllPrecentral gyrus3 to 10.5 months; 18 months to 20 to 24 yearsPrecentral gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 weeks to 2, 15 monthsSupramarginal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months
67C6ParietalAllPostcentral gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 yearsPostcentral gyrus3 to 20 to 24 years
Temporal2 weeks to 12 yearsRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)12 years; 20 to 24 yearsSuperior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 2 years; 20 to 24 years
Superior temporal gyrus, central part1 months to 2 yearsSupramarginal gyrus20 to 24 years
68CP5ParietalAllPosterior temporal lobeAllAngular gyrus4.5, 9 months to 20 to 24 years
Temporal2 weeks to 12 yearsRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)AllMiddle temporal gyrus2 weeks to 2 months, 4 years
Superior temporal gyrus6, 7.5, 12, 18 months to 4 years
Supramarginal gyrus6, 7.5, 10.5, 12, 18 months; 12, 20 to 24 years
69CP3ParietalAllRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)AllAngular gyrusAll
Supramarginal gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
70CP1ParietalAllRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 weeksPostcentral gyrus10.5 months; 20 to 24 years
Superior parietal gyrusAllSuperior parietal gyrusAll
71CP2ParietalAllPostcentral gyrus6 monthsPostcentral gyrus6 months
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)1 monthSuperior parietal gyrusAll
Superior parietal gyrusAll
72CP4ParietalAllRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)AllAngular gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months; 7.5 months to 20 to 24 years
Supramarginal gyrus1 month to 20 to 24 years
73CP6ParietalAllPosterior temporal lobe2 weeks to 12 yearsMiddle temporal gyrus2 weeks; 1 month
Temporal2 weeks to 12 yearsRemainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)AllSuperior temporal gyrus2 weeks to 12 years
Supramarginal gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
74P5ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4 years; 20 to 24 yearsAngular gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Posterior temporal lobe2 weeks to 7.5 months; 12, 18 months, 4 yearsMiddle occipital gyrusAll
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 months to 20 to 24 years
75P3ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4 yearsAngular gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 months to 20 to 24 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus2 weeks to 9, 12 months to 4 years
76P1ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4.5 months; 9, 12, 15 months, 4 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1 month
Superior parietal gyrus2 monthss to 20 to 24 yearsSuperior occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1 month
Superior parietal gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
77P2ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4.5 months; 9, 12, 15 monthsAngular gyrus12 months; 2 years
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1 month
Superior parietal gyrus2 weeks; 2 months to 20 to 24 yearsSuperior occipital gyrus2 weeks; 1 month
Superior parietal gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
78P4ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4.5 months; 9 to 18 monthsAngular gyrusAll
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)AllMiddle occipital gyrus2 weeks to 3, 15 months
79P6ParietalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobe2 weeks to 4.5 months; 9 months to 4 years; 20 to 24 yearsAngular gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
Posterior temporal lobe2 weeks to 7.5 months; 10.5 months to 2 yearsMiddle occipital gyrus2 weeks to 4.5 months; 7.5 months to 4 years
Remainder of parietal lobe (including supramarginal and angular gyrus)2 months to 20 to 24 years
80PO3OccipitalAllLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllMiddle occipital gyrusAll
Parietal2 months to 20 to 24 yearsSuperior occipital gyrus2 months to 20 to 24 years
81PO4Cerebellum2 weeks, 2 monthsLateral remainder of occipital lobeAllMiddle occipital gyrusAll
OccipitalAllSuperior occipital gyrus4.5 to 7.5 months; 10.5, 15, 18 months; 4 to 20 to 24 years
Parietal2 months to 20 to 24 years

Fig. 8

Age-group differences in the correspondence between scalp channel location and the underlying brain ROI from the lobar, Hammer, and LPBA40 atlas. Scalp 10–10 channel locations are overlaid on the average template for age 2 years. Channel locations colored in green were mapped to the same ROI(s) across all age groups. Blue indicates that the channel location was mapped to at least one ROI across age groups, and red shows that different ROIs were corresponded to the channel location across age groups. The information on between-age consistency was based on Table 2.

NPH_8_3_035003_f008.png

The Hammer and LPBA40 atlases have smaller structural segmentations than the lobar atlas. Hence, most of the channel locations were mapped to more than one ROI. Seventy-four pairs of channel-to-Hammer-ROI mapping across 67 channel locations were consistent across all age groups. Sixty-seven pairs of channel-to-LPBA40-ROI mapping across 60 channel locations were identical for all ages. Many channels had at least one identical ROI mapped to the location for all age groups across both atlases. These included channels that were sensitive to the superior frontal gyrus (FPZ, AFZ, FZ, FCZ, F2, FC1, and FC2), middle frontal gyrus (FP1, FP2, AF3, AF4, F5, F3, F4, F6, FC3, and FC4), inferior frontal gyrus (F7, F8, F5, F6, FC5, and FC6), precentral gyrus (FC5, C1, and C2), postcentral gyrus (C3 and C4), superior parietal gyrus (CPZ, CP1, and CP2), angular gyrus (CP3 and P4), cuneus (POZ), inferior temporal gyrus (FT9, T9, and T10), middle temporal gyrus (T7 and T8), and cerebellum (TP9, P9, PO9, I1, TP10, P10, PO10, and I2) across all age groups. However, between-age differences in channel-to-ROI correspondence were also observed for Hammer and LPBA40 ROI parcellations. Fourteen channel locations did not consistently map to a Hammer ROI across age groups. There were 21 channels with no consistent channel-to-LPBA40-ROI mapping across age groups.

Figures 9 and 10 provide examples of the channel locations sensitive to ROIs delineated using LPBA40. Figure 9 shows a consistent pattern of channel locations across age groups for the inferior frontal gyrus, AF7, F5, F7, FC5, AF8, F6, F8, and FC6; channel location F3 also was sensitive to the inferior frontal gyrus at age 12 months. Figure 10 shows a less consistent pattern of channel locations sensitive to the postcentral gyrus, including CPz, CP1, C1, C3, C4, C5, and C6. However, the pattern of sensitive channel locations was different for each age.

Fig. 9

S-D channel locations sensitive to the inferior frontal gyrus. The left inferior frontal gyrus is shown on the head models. Examples of the scalp-to-ROI mapping using S-D channel DOT estimation were displayed in selected age groups (3 months, 12 months, 12 years, and 20 to 24 years). ROIs are delineated using the LONI Probabilistic Brain Atlas (LPBA40).62 Channel locations are displayed on age-matched average templates.

NPH_8_3_035003_f009.png

Fig. 10

S-D channel locations sensitive to the postcentral gyrus. Examples of the scalp-to-ROI mapping using S-D channel DOT estimation were displayed in selected age groups (3 months, 12 months, 12 years, and 20 to 24 years). ROIs are delineated using the LONI Probabilistic Brain Atlas (LPBA40).62 Channel locations are displayed on age-matched average templates.

NPH_8_3_035003_f010.png

4.

Discussion

This study examined age differences in scalp-to-cortex distances and scalp-location-to-ROI correspondence across infancy (2 weeks to 24 months) and compared these measures to child (4, 12 years) and adult age groups (20 to 24 years). We extended existing coregistration and photon migration simulation methods to realistic head models. The use of DOT sensitivity estimated from photon migration simulations to depict scalp-to-cortex correspondence has direct applications for NIRS and fNIRS research. Our S-D channel DOT estimations revealed the scalp-to-cortex distances and underlying brain regions that are measurable with NIRS instruments across age groups. All three estimation methods showed that the scalp-to-cortex distances increased from infancy to childhood and adulthood. There were considerable variations in the distance measures among the infant age groups. The probabilistic mappings between the scalp locations and cortical lobes were relatively stable across development. However, the mappings between the scalp locations and cortical ROIs in sublobar atlas parcellations showed greater age-related variations. We found that individual participant MRIs and average MRI templates from the Neurodevelopmental MRI Database2629 were similar in their scalp-to-cortex distances. This study has the key implication of highlighting the importance of using age-appropriate realistic head models to estimate DOT sensitivity that can be used to facilitate anatomical interpretations of NIRS and fNIRS data.

4.1.

Scalp-to-Cortex Distance

Three methods were used to measure scalp-to-cortex distances. The scalp projection method has been commonly adopted in developmental and adult studies to measure the anatomical distance between the scalp electrode locations and the cortical surface.13,1720 We also estimated the distance between the scalp location and the maximum fluence for the “direct DOT” fluence distribution. The distances for these two measures were very similar. This is likely due to the monotonic and somewhat nonlinear photon decay distribution across the head which is maximal near the surface of the cortex.37 The S-D channel DOT fluence distances were greater than distances estimated from the other methods across scalp locations for all age groups. The S-D channel DOT fluence represents the flow of photons from the source to the detector37 and was deeper than the cortical surface (scalp projection) or the photon flow from the optode source injection point (direct DOT). However, it is important to note that spatial projection, direct DOT, and S-D channel DOT are three distinctive methods for interrogating scalp-to-cortex correspondence. The scalp projection method measures the anatomical relation between the electrode and cortical locations. The DOT sensitivity methods describe the mapping between optode or channel locations and the underlying cortical regions that can be sampled by DOT instruments.

All three estimation methods indicated that scalp-to-cortex distances averaged across the scalp locations increased from infancy to childhood and from childhood to adulthood. This finding is consistent with Ref. 17 who measured whole-brain distances from the scalp in newborns through 12-year-olds and other studies that measured scalp-to-cortex distances by electrode positions (22- to 51-year-olds,13 3.4- to 16.3-week-olds,18 and 5- to 11-year-olds20). The increase in scalp-to-cortex distances from infancy to adulthood may be attributed to several types of changes in the structure of the head. These include skull thickness,69,70 increases in CSF volume,16 global brain volume and GM/WM growth,8,9,15,16 and cortical folding during the first 2 years.71 Brain structural growth is more gradual during childhood and adolescence and reaches a plateau during adulthood.8,9,72,73

There were between-electrode variations in scalp-to-cortex distance, and these variations were heterogeneous among infant groups. The scalp projection and S-D channel DOT distances for all age groups were greatest at the frontal and central electrodes on the bottom row around the face area, followed by electrode locations at the midline along the intrahemispherical fissure, and the smallest on the electrode locations internal to these edges. Such interelectrode variations were also found in previous studies.13,18,20 Furthermore, we extended prior findings with young infants18 and revealed that there was a prominent decrease in distances from anterior to posterior locations in most of the infant groups (scalp projection distances in all infant ages and S-D channel DOT distances in 3-month to 15-month-olds). This anterior-to-posterior decrease in distance was less discernible in older age groups including adults measured using scalp projection.13 The anterior–posterior gradient coincides with the posterior-to-anterior sequence of cortical maturation.73,74 The occipital and parietal lobes show faster growth in volume during infancy than the frontal regions.74 This implies that the posterior regions have expanded closer to the scalp surface earlier than anterior regions. The region-specific heterogeneous patterns of brain development that are observable from infancy15,16,7476 may contribute to the lack of systematic age-related increase in scalp-to-cortex distances among infant groups.

Hemispheric asymmetry in scalp-to-cortex distances was also observed. The spatial projection estimation showed that the scalp-to-cortex distances were larger in the right than the left hemisphere across age groups. This was consistent with the published finding and could be attributed to cortical morphometry.17 Greater growth in the left hemisphere may have reduced the distance between the skull and cortex surface. In contrast, the S-D channel DOT estimation revealed greater distances in the left than the right hemisphere for some infant groups, as well as children and adults. One possible interpretation is that greater S-D channel DOT fluence strength might be carried deeper into the cortex in the left than right hemisphere in these age groups. An in-depth investigation into the estimation-method-related differences in hemispheric asymmetry is outside the scope of this study. Our findings highlight that the scalp-to-cortex distances are sensitive to the complex age- and region-dependent cortical growth. Further investigations are needed to assess the hemispheric and regional differences in scalp-to-brain distances using the DOT sensitivity methods.

Gauging scalp-to-cortex distance is a foundational step for optimizing DOT sensitivity to the target cortical regions. Fu and Richards37 showed that the infant groups displayed different DOT sensitivity profiles compared with the adults with source–detector channels placed at the same distances. A common practice is to use longer separations for adults than infants.77,78 Increasing the separation distance allows the fluence distribution to extend deeper into the head tissues and thus sample cortical regions at greater depth.35,37 However, the increased depth sensitivity is at the expense of decreased signal strength.79 The current findings on age differences in the scalp-to-cortex distance can be used with age-specific DOT sensitivity profiles37 to find optimal source–detector separation distances that allow for comparable depth sensitivity for different age groups.

Age-appropriate MRI templates may be used to describe cranio-cerebral correspondence when subjects’ own MRIs are unavailable. This study showed that the age-appropriate templates have comparable S-D channel DOT distances as the individual head models. Furthermore, the S-D channel DOT distance estimations were robust across different Monte Carlo simulation methods (MCX, MMC, and tMCimg) for 3-month and 6-month infants’ individual MRIs and age-appropriate average templates (Figs. S5–S7 in the Supplemental Material). The developmental differences in scalp-to-cortex distances suggest that adult head models should not be used to make anatomical inferences for infant or child standalone NIRS/fNIRS data. We recommend using age-appropriate average templates or the individual MRIs when the subject’s own MRIs are not collected.19,32,33

4.2.

Scalp-Location-to-ROI Mapping

This is the first study that provided scalp-location-to-ROI look-up tables (see Supplemental Table S3) computed using multiple methods for a wide range of age groups. The full look-up tables provide references for NIRS/fNIRS researchers to design optode configurations based on their study-specific age groups and ROIs. The tables can also be used to evaluate channel-to-ROI mapping for existing data. In addition, the channel-to-ROI mapping estimations can be read into programs or toolboxes to provide a user-friendly and customizable tool for optode placement optimization. The application of our estimations is further discussed below.

We used the scalp projection and the S-D channel DOT fluence to map 10–10 scalp electrode/channel locations with underlying ROIs from a macrostructural atlas (lobar) and two sublobar atlases (Hammers and LPBA40). The spatial projection method mapped underlying ROIs based only on the spatial anatomical relations. The S-D channel DOT method mapped underlying ROIs based on the DOT sensitivity profile. The S-D channel-DOT look-up table supported the prior finding that there is consistent correspondence between the majority of scalp locations and macrostructural ROIs for infants18,25 and adults.13,14 For smaller sublobar atlas ROIs, some channel locations show consistent correspondence between scalp location and underlying cortical ROI. For example, F5 and F6 were corresponded to bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyrus in all age groups. However, many channel locations show inconstant mappings across age groups. These include posterior midline positions and frontal channels on the bottom row where the scalp-to-cortex distance was larger. For example, channel TP7 was sensitive to both the inferior and middle temporal gyrus in the LPBA40 atlas for age 2 weeks through 12 years whereas it was mapped to only the middle temporal gyrus for 20- to 24-year-olds. Hemispheric symmetry/asymmetry in the mapping between channel locations and sublobar atlas ROIs was also observed. For example, the channel locations that were sensitive to the postcentral gyrus were symmetric between hemispheres at age 2 weeks, 1 month, 4.5 month, 9 month, 15 months, and 12 years but showed asymmetry for the other age groups. The observed age-related changes in hemispheric asymmetry might be related to asymmetry in the GM growth across development.74 Our findings further underscored the importance of using age-appropriate head models to fully account for the age-dependent head and cortical structural changes when examining craniocerebral correspondence.

The age-appropriate S-D channel DOT look-up procedures can be adopted to localize the ROI(s) that generate the fNIRS activities and design the optode arrangement prior to data collection. Our findings highlight a recognized issue in fNIRS data interpretation: the channel(s) that show significant activations from the group analysis may not correspond to the same ROI for all participants.80 This problem is especially concerning for infant and child studies that encompass a wide age range. Our S-D channel DOT look-up procedures provide an effective solution. The optode locations recorded from an experiment can be coregistered with individual head models from infants closely matched in age and head measurements if subjects’ own MRIs are not available. S-D channel DOT sensitivity is then estimated to infer the subject-specific ROI(s) that have generated the fNIRS signals. The methodological details were also presented elsewhere.3941

Our method and look-up tables may also be used to design optode placement on NIRS holders that maximize channel sensitivity to hemodynamic changes of underlying ROIs. The tables we provide for the 10–10 or 10–5 recording system can be used as look-up tables for either manual construction of optode locations or used with automatic methods. We additionally provided an age-appropriate S-D channel DOT look-up table that quantifies the sensitivity of channel pairings from the fOLD toolbox42 to lobar and sublobar ROIs ( Supplemental Table S4). The table provides the specificity (%) of the channel to the ROI out of the total S-D channel DOT sensitivity to all ROIs in the atlas. Researchers can thus design their holders to include channels that are sensitive to the user-specified ROIs with a specificity threshold.42 Figure S2(d) in the Supplemental Material shows an example of optode placement that includes channels sensitive to the left inferior frontal gyrus with specificity greater than 1% for 6-month infants. We have recently developed the devfOLD toolbox based on the existing fOLD toolbox to facilitate age-specific design of optode placement that maximizes channel-to-ROI sensitivity.81

4.3.

Implications

The age-specific scalp-to-cortex distances and channel-to-ROI look-up tables based on the S-D channel DOT estimations can be used to guide NIRS and fNIRS channel placements and data analysis. Cross-sectional and longitudinal fNIRS studies need to ensure that age-group comparisons are made on data from sensors that sample the same ROI with comparable sensitivity to the cortex across ages. For example, for a study aiming to compare activations in the inferior frontal gyrus among 3-month, 12-month, and 12-year-old participants, Supplemental Table S3 and Fig. 11 indicate that sensor placement that covers channel locations AF7, F7, F5, F3, FC5, AF8, F8, F6, and FC6 could be used for all age groups. We also know that the average distances to the cortex from the frontal electrode locations differed by age (3 months: 8 mm; 12 months: 6 mm; 12 years: 10 mm). Hence, larger source–detector separation distances are expected for older ages. fNIRS studies conventionally use 20 to 30 mm separations for infants77,82 and 30 to 35 mm for children, e.g., Refs. 83 and 84. Experimenters should measure the source–detector separation distances after constructing a preliminary holder and adjust accordingly. Experimenters are not advised to compare activations at F3 channel locations between 3-month and 12-month-olds, as F3 may not sample activities from the left inferior frontal gyrus in 3-month-olds.

4.4.

Limitations

This study did not compute the thickness of segmentation layers or scalp-to-cortex distances by ROI parcellations. This means that we cannot pinpoint the particular tissue layer(s) or cortical region(s) that may contribute to the age-related changes in scalp-to-cortex distance as done in Ref. 17. Furthermore, existing evidence indicated that DOT sensitivity as a function of source–detector separation distance was different in GM, WM, CSF, and extracerebral tissues (scalp and skull) in infants and adults.34,35,85,86 Future analyses that compare distances from the scalp by tissue types and ROIs could more precisely inform age-related differences in the optimal separation distances for sampling cortical signal changes at different brain regions.

Differences in data collection sites may contribute to the differences in data collection sites. We included MRIs from a wide range of data sources to increase the generalizability of our results on scalp-to-cortex correspondence. However, the number of participants in each age group was not evenly distributed across databases. This was partly due to the design and the specific age groups targeted in the individual database. Hence, we cannot tease apart the effect of age and database in this study. We examined the effect of age, estimation method, and electrode locations with databased as a categorical covariate variable. The analyses are presented in the Supplemental Material. The interaction effect of the three factors remained after controlling for database difference. However, the specific pattern of age-related differences across infant age groups varied with the covariate added to the model. With the expansion of the Neurodevelopmental MRI Database, our future study aims to balance the number of participants from each group across the databases.

5.

Conclusions

This study examined the scalp-to-cortex distances and scalp-to-cortical ROI correspondence in infants, children, and adults using both the scalp projection methods and DOT sensitivity estimations. There were differences in the scalp-to-cortex distance in infants, which become magnified in children and adults. We found systematic differences in scalp-location-to-anatomical ROI correspondence for different ages, both for spatial projection and for DOT sensitivity. Our findings imply that accurate anatomical interpretations of NIRS/fNIRS data are dependent on developmentally sensitive estimations of DOT sensitivity that account for the head and cortical development. This study demonstrated that age-appropriate realistic head models should be used to provide anatomical guidance for standalone DOT data.

Disclosures

X. Fu and J. E. Richards have no relevant financial interests in the manuscript and no other potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD-R01-HD18942, NICHD-R37-HD18942, NIHCD-R03-HD091464) to J. E. Richards.

Code, Data, and Materials Availability

The age-specific average templates, including T1, DOT sensitivity average template can be accessed from the “Neurodevelopmental MRI Database.” The database is available online: http://jerlab.psych.sc.edu/NeurodevelopmentalMRIDatabase/ for information and https://www.nitrc.org/projects/neurodevdata/for access. Details for the online access are provided in Ref. 27. The codes that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, XF, upon request.

References

1. 

T. J. Huppert, M. A. Franceschini, D. A. Boas, “Noninvasive Imaging of cerebral activation with diffuse optical tomography,” In Vivo Optical Imaging of Brain Function, 2nd ed.CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton, Florida (2009). Google Scholar

2. 

B. Chance et al., “Optical imaging of brain function and metabolism,” J. Neurol., 239 (7), 359 –360 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00812149 Google Scholar

3. 

A. Villringer and B. Chance, “Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function,” Trends Neurosci., 20 (10), 435 –442 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01132-6 TNSCDR 0166-2236 Google Scholar

4. 

D. Tsuzuki and I. Dan, “Spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: from channel position on the scalp to cortical location in individual and group analyses,” NeuroImage, 85 92 –103 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.025 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

5. 

D. A. Boas et al., “Three dimensional Monte Carlo code for photon migration through complex heterogeneous media including the adult human head,” Opt. Express, 10 159 –170 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1364/OE.10.000159 OPEXFF 1094-4087 Google Scholar

6. 

R. J. Cooper et al., “Validating atlas-guided DOT: a comparison of diffuse optical tomography informed by atlas and subject-specific anatomies,” NeuroImage, 62 (3), 1999 –2006 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.031 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

7. 

J. P. Culver et al., “Volumetric diffuse optical tomography of brain activity,” Opt. Lett., 28 (21), 2061 –2063 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1364/OL.28.002061 OPLEDP 0146-9592 Google Scholar

8. 

J. E. Richards, W. Xie, “Brains for all the ages: structural neurodevelopment in infants and children from a life-span perspective,” Advances in Child Development and Beahvior, 48 2015). Google Scholar

9. 

J. E. Richards, S. Conte, “Brain development in infants: structure and experience,” Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2020). Google Scholar

10. 

S. Brigadoi et al., “A 4D neonatal head model for diffuse optical imaging of pre-term to term infants,” NeuroImage, 100 385 –394 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.028 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

11. 

V. Jurcak, D. Tsuzuki and I. Dan, “10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems,” NeuroImage, 34 (4), 1600 –1611 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.024 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

12. 

M. Okamoto and I. Dan, “Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping,” NeuroImage, 26 (1), 18 –28 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.018 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

13. 

M. Okamoto et al., “Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping,” NeuroImage, 21 99 –111 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.026 Google Scholar

14. 

L. Koessler et al., “Automated cortical projection of EEG sensors: anatomical correlation via the international 10–10 system,” NeuroImage, 46 (1), 64 –72 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.006 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

15. 

J. H. Gilmore et al., “Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years,” Cereb. Cortex, 22 (11), 2478 –2485 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr327 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

16. 

A. Makropoulos et al., “Regional growth and atlasing of the developing human brain,” NeuroImage, 125 456 –478 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.047 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

17. 

M. S. Beauchamp et al., “The developmental trajectory of brain-scalp distance from birth through childhood: implications for functional neuroimaging,” PLoS One, 6 (9), e24981 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024981 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

18. 

C. Kabdebon et al., “Anatomical correlations of the international 10–20 sensor placement system in infants,” NeuroImage, 99 342 –356 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.046 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

19. 

L. L. Emberson et al., “The lateral occipital cortex is selective for object shape, not texture/color, at six months,” J. Neurosci., 37 (13), 3698 –3703 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-16.2017 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

20. 

A. Whiteman et al., “Investigation of the sensitivity of functional near-infrared spectroscopy brain imaging to anatomical variations in 5- to 11-year-old children,” Neurophotonics, 5 (1), 011009 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.5.1.011009 Google Scholar

21. 

T. Fekete et al., “A stand-alone method for anatomical localization of NIRS measurements,” NeuroImage, 56 (4), 2080 –2088 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.068 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

22. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Stable and convenient spatial registration of stand-alone NIRS data through anchor-based probabilistic registration,” Neurosci. Res., 72 (2), 163 –171 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.10.008 Google Scholar

23. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Macroanatomical landmarks featuring junctions of major sulci and fissures and scalp landmarks based on the international 10–10 system for analyzing lateral cortical development of infants,” Front. Neurosci., 11 394 (2017). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00394 1662-453X Google Scholar

24. 

M. Matsui et al., “Referential framework for transcranial anatomical correspondence for fNIRS based on manually traced sulci and gyri of an infant brain,” Neurosci. Res., 80 55 –68 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2014.01.003 Google Scholar

25. 

S. Lloyd-Fox et al., “Coregistering functional near-infrared spectroscopy with underlying cortical areas in infants,” Neurophotonics, 1 (2), 025006 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.1.2.025006 Google Scholar

26. 

J. E. Richards, “Updating the neurodevelopmental MRI database,” Google Scholar

27. 

J. E. Richards et al., “A database of age-appropriate average MRI templates,” NeuroImage, 124 (Pt B), 1254 –1259 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.055 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

28. 

C. E. Sanchez, J. E. Richards and C. R. Almli, “Neurodevelopmental MRI brain templates for children from 2 weeks to 4 years of age (vol 54, pg 77, 2012),” Dev. Psychobiol., 54 (4), 474 –474 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21018 DEPBA5 0012-1630 Google Scholar

29. 

C. E. Sanchez, J. E. Richards and C. R. Almli, “Age-specific MRI templates for pediatric neuroimaging,” Dev. Neuropsychol., 37 (5), 379 –399 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2012.688900 DENEE8 Google Scholar

30. 

P. T. Fillmore et al., “Stereotaxic magnetic resonance imaging brain atlases for infants from 3 to 12 months,” Dev. Neurosci., 37 (6), 515 –532 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1159/000438749 Google Scholar

31. 

S. Lloyd-Fox et al., “Cortical activation to action perception is associated with action production abilities in young infants,” Cereb. Cortex, 25 (2), 289 –297 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht207 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

32. 

L. L. Emberson et al., “Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci., 23 26 –38 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.002 Google Scholar

33. 

L. L. Emberson, J. E. Richards and R. N. Aslin, “Top-down modulation in the infant brain: learning-induced expectations rapidly affect the sensory cortex at 6 months,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 112 (31), 9585 –9590 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510343112 Google Scholar

34. 

C. Mansouri et al., “Depth sensitivity analysis of functional near-infrared spectroscopy measurement using three-dimensional Monte Carlo modeling-based magnetic resonance imaging,” Lasers Med. Sci., 25 (3), 431 –438 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10103-010-0754-4 Google Scholar

35. 

G. Strangman, Z. Li and Q. Zhang, “Depth sensitivity and source-detector separations for near infrared spectroscopy based on the Colin27 brain template,” PLoS One, 8 (8), e66319 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066319 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

36. 

S. Brigadoi and R. J. Cooper, “How short is short? Optimum source-detector distance for short-separation channels in functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurophotonics, 2 (2), 025005 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.2.2.025005 Google Scholar

37. 

X. Fu and J. E. Richards, “Age-related changes in diffuse optical tomography sensitivity by source-detector separation in infancy,” PLoS One, 16 (6), e0252036 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252036 Google Scholar

38. 

F. B. Haeussinger et al., “Simulation of near-infrared light absorption considering individual head and prefrontal cortex anatomy: implications for optical neuroimaging,” PLoS One, 6 (10), e26377 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026377 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

39. 

C. Bulgarelli et al., “Fronto-temporoparietal connectivity and self-awareness in 18-month-olds: a resting state fNIRS study,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci., 38 100676 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100676 Google Scholar

40. 

C. Bulgarelli et al., “The developmental trajectory of fronto-temporoparietal connectivity as a proxy of the default mode network: a longitudinal fNIRS investigation,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 41 (10), 2717 –2740 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24974 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

41. 

K. L. Perdue et al., “Using functional near-infrared spectroscopy to assess social information processing in poor urban Bangladeshi infants and toddlers,” Dev. Sci., 22 (5), e12839 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12839 Google Scholar

42. 

G. A. Zimeo Morais, J. B. Balardin and J. R. Sato, “fNIRS optodes’ location decider (fOLD): a toolbox for probe arrangement guided by brain regions-of-interest,” Sci. Rep., 8 (1), 3341 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21716-z SRCEC3 2045-2322 Google Scholar

43. 

S. Brigadoi et al., “Array designer: automated optimized array design for functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurophotonics, 5 (3), 035010 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.5.3.035010 Google Scholar

44. 

A. Di Martino et al., “The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism,” Mol. Psychiatry, 19 (6), 659 –667 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2013.78 Google Scholar

45. 

B. R. Howell et al., “The UNC/UMN Baby Connectome Project (BCP): an overview of the study design and protocol development,” NeuroImage, 185 891 –905 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.049 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

46. 

J. H. Gilmore et al., “Genetic and environmental contributions to neonatal brain structure: a twin study,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 31 (8), 1174 –1182 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20926 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

47. 

H. C. Hazlett et al., “Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder,” Nature, 542 (7641), 348 –351 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature21369 Google Scholar

48. 

T. L. Jernigan et al., “The pediatric imaging, neurocognition, and genetics (PING) data repository,” NeuroImage, 124 1149 –1154 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.057 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

49. 

P. T. Fillmore, M. C. Phillips-Meek and J. E. Richards, “Age-specific MRI brain and head templates for healthy adults from 20 through 89 years of age,” Front. Aging Neurosci., 7 44 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00044 Google Scholar

50. 

B. B. Avants et al., “A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration,” NeuroImage, 54 (3), 2033 –2044 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

51. 

N. J. Tustison et al., “N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction,” IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging, 29 (6), 1310 –1320 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908 ITMID4 0278-0062 Google Scholar

52. 

G. Douaud et al., “Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia,” Brain, 130 (9), 2375 –2386 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm184 BRAIAK 0006-8950 Google Scholar

53. 

M. Jenkinson, M. Pechaud and S. M. Smith, “BET2: MR-based estimation of brain, skull and scalp surfaces,” in Eleventh Annu. Meeting of the Org. for Hum. Brain Mapp., (2005). Google Scholar

54. 

S. M. Smith et al., “Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL,” NeuroImage, 23 S208 –S219 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

55. 

Y. Zhang, M. Brady and S. Smith, “Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm,” IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging, 20 45 –57 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906424 Google Scholar

56. 

C. Rorden, “MRIcroGL,” (2012). http://wwwmccauslandcenterscedu/mricrogl/ Google Scholar

57. 

C. Rorden and M. Brett, “Stereotaxic display of brain lesions,” Behav. Neurol., 12 421719 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1155/2000/421719 BNEUEI Google Scholar

58. 

Q. Fang and D. A. Boas, “Tetrahedral mesh generation from volumetric binary and grayscale images,” in IEEE Int. Symp. Biomed. Imaging: From Nano to Macro, (2009). Google Scholar

59. 

S. Yan, A. P. Tran and Q. Fang, “Dual-grid mesh-based Monte Carlo algorithm for efficient photon transport simulations in complex three-dimensional media,” J. Biomed. Opt., 24 (2), 020503 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.24.2.020503 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

60. 

J. E. Richards et al., “Evaluating methods for constructing average high-density electrode positions,” Brain Topogr., 28 (1), 70 –86 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-014-0400-8 BRTOEZ 0896-0267 Google Scholar

61. 

J. E. Richards, “Cortical sources of ERP in prosaccade and antisaccade eye movements using realistic source models,” Front. Syst. Neurosci., 7 27 (2013). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00027 Google Scholar

62. 

D. W. Shattuck et al., “Construction of a 3D probabilistic atlas of human cortical structures,” NeuroImage, 39 (3), 1064 –1080 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.031 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

63. 

R. A. Heckemann et al., “Automatic anatomical brain MRI segmentation combining label propagation and decision fusion,” NeuroImage, 33 (1), 115 –126 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.061 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

64. 

I. S. Gousias et al., “Automatic segmentation of brain MRIs of 2-year-olds into 83 regions of interest,” NeuroImage, 40 (2), 672 –684 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.034 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

65. 

M. Jenkinson and S. Smith, “A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images,” Med. Image Anal., 5 (2), 143 –156 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6 Google Scholar

66. 

Q. Fang and S. Yan, “Graphics processing unit-accelerated mesh-based Monte Carlo photon transport simulations,” J. Biomed. Opt., 24 (11), 115002 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.24.11.115002 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

67. 

Q. Fang and D. A. Boas, “Monte Carlo simulation of photon migration in 3D turbid media accelerated by graphics processing units,” Opt. Express, 17 (22), 20178 –20190 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1364/OE.17.020178 OPEXFF 1094-4087 Google Scholar

68. 

A. P. Tran, S. Yan and Q. Fang, “Improving model-based functional near-infrared spectroscopy analysis using mesh-based anatomical and light-transport models,” Neurophotonics, 7 (1), 015008 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.7.1.015008 Google Scholar

69. 

L. L. Emberson et al., “Isolating the effects of surface vasculature in infant neuroimaging using short-distance optical channels: a combination of local and global effects,” Neurophotonics, 3 (3), 031406 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.3.3.031406 Google Scholar

70. 

C. F. Hansman, “Growth of interorbital distance and skull thickness as observed in roentgenographic measurements,” Radiology, 86 (1), 87 –96 (1966). https://doi.org/10.1148/86.1.87 RADLAX 0033-8419 Google Scholar

71. 

G. Li et al., “Mapping longitudinal development of local cortical gyrification in infants from birth to 2 years of age,” J. Neurosci., 34 (12), 4228 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3976-13.2014 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

72. 

A. Raznahan et al., “How does your cortex grow?,” J. Neurosci., 31 (19), 7174 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-11.2011 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

73. 

Brain Development Cooperative Group, “Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI study of normal brain development,” Cereb. Cortex, 22 (1), 1 –12 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr018 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

74. 

J. H. Gilmore et al., “Regional gray matter growth, sexual dimorphism, and cerebral asymmetry in the neonatal brain,” J. Neurosci., 27 (6), 1255 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3339-06.2007 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

75. 

G. Li et al., “Spatial patterns, longitudinal development, and hemispheric asymmetries of cortical thickness in infants from birth to 2 years of age,” J. Neurosci., 35 (24), 9150 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4107-14.2015 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

76. 

J. Remer et al., “Quantifying cortical development in typically developing toddlers and young children, 1–6 years of age,” NeuroImage, 153 246 –261 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.010 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

77. 

J. Gervain et al., “Near-infrared spectroscopy: a report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci., 1 (1), 22 –46 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.004 Google Scholar

78. 

P. Pinti et al., “The present and future use of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for cognitive neuroscience,” Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1464 (1), 5 –29 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13948 ANYAA9 0077-8923 Google Scholar

79. 

L. Wang, H. Ayaz and M. Izzetoglu, “Investigation of the source-detector separation in near infrared spectroscopy for healthy and clinical applications,” J. Biophotonics, 12 (11), e201900175 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1002/jbio.201900175 Google Scholar

80. 

L. J. Powell, B. Deen and R. Saxe, “Using individual functional channels of interest to study cortical development with fNIRS,” Dev. Sci., 21 (4), e12595 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12595 Google Scholar

81. 

X. Fu and J. E. Richards, “Age-specific design of fNIRS channel placement: devfOLD: the developmental fNIRS optodes’ location decider,” Google Scholar

82. 

S. Lloyd-Fox, A. Blasi and C. E. Elwell, “Illuminating the developing brain: the past, present and future of functional near infrared spectroscopy,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 34 (3), 269 –284 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.008 NBREDE 0149-7634 Google Scholar

83. 

X. P. Ding, G. Fu and K. Lee, “Neural correlates of own- and other-race face recognition in children: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study,” NeuroImage, 85 335 –344 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.051 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

84. 

S. B. Perlman et al., “fNIRS evidence of prefrontal regulation of frustration in early childhood,” NeuroImage, 85 326 –334 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.057 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

85. 

G. Strangman, Q. Zhang and Z. Li, “Scalp and skull influence on near infrared photon propagation in the Colin27 brain template,” NeuroImage, 85 136 –149 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.090 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

86. 

Y. Fukui, Y. Ajichi and E. Okada, “Monte Carlo prediction of near-infrared light propagation in realistic adult and neonatal head models,” Appl. Opt., 42 (16), 002881 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.42.002881 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

Biography

Xiaoxue Fu is a postdoctoral fellow at the Psychology Department of University of South Carolina. She received her PhD in developmental psychology at the Pennsylvania State University. Her postdoctoral work focuses on cortical source localization methodology for fNIRS and ERP.

John E. Richards is a research professor at the Psychology Department of University of South Carolina. He has expertise in applying image reconstruction techniques to fNIRS data using age-specific realistic head models. He is also specialized in studying brain sources of the ERP related to attention. He developed and maintains the Neurodevelopmental MRI database, which contains structural MRIs from over 11,000 individuals ranging from two weeks to 89 years.

CC BY: © The Authors. Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI.
Xiaoxue Fu and John E. Richards "Investigating developmental changes in scalp-to-cortex correspondence using diffuse optical tomography sensitivity in infancy," Neurophotonics 8(3), 035003 (24 July 2021). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.8.3.035003
Received: 15 December 2020; Accepted: 9 July 2021; Published: 24 July 2021
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 10 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
KEYWORDS
Electrodes

Magnetic resonance imaging

Head

Brain

Cerebellum

Neurophotonics

Near infrared spectroscopy

Back to Top